Approaches in research on flood risk perception and their importance in flood risk management: a review

  • Original Paper
  • Open access
  • Published: 15 December 2021
  • Volume 111 , pages 2343–2378, ( 2022 )

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

research paper on flood management

  • Ewa Lechowska   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-2588-1206 1  

9843 Accesses

25 Citations

Explore all metrics

The study of flood risk perception factors can be considered by using different paradigms. In an attempt to understand risk perception, two basic paradigms can be distinguished: rationalist and constructivist. The rationalist approach tends to focus on modeling, characterizing, and predicting behavioral results regarding various threats. According to the constructivist paradigm, threats are perceived as socially constructed. This review paper aims to assess the importance of the rationalist and constructivist approaches in research on flood risk perception and flood risk management more broadly by answering the questions: (1) Which paradigm dominates the research of flood risk perception?, (2) What is the relationship between rationalistic and constructivistic factors (e.g., stimulation, weakening, strengthening, etc.)?, (3) which factors are more effective in moderating attitudes toward flood risk? The paper concludes by pointing out the desired direction of research on flood risk perception from the perspective of improving flood risk management. In contemporary empirical works managing the perception of flood risk, a rationalistic approach that psychometrically searches for cognitive models dominates. Often, statistically obtained dependencies are mutually exclusive. Studies on perception that apply the constructivist approach are in an early stage of development, nevertheless providing consistent results. They indicate that the social, political, cultural, religious, and historical contexts shape the perception of flood risk. On the basis of the aforementioned information, research on flood risk in a constructivist approach should be expanded, as it provides a clear, often underappreciated catalog of contextual factors shaping risk perception and, importantly, simultaneously moderating the influence of rationalist factors on flood risk perception.

Similar content being viewed by others

research paper on flood management

Introduction – Living with Perils in the Twenty-First Century

research paper on flood management

Interpretive Environmental Risk Research: Affect, Discourses and Change

research paper on flood management

Potential Linkages Between Social Capital, Flood Risk Perceptions, and Self-Efficacy

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

1 Introduction

Traditional approaches to flood risk management (FRM) that focus on physical flood protection or improvements in flood monitoring and forecasting have tended to overlook the social dimensions of floods, such as the public’s understanding of risk. Risk perception, which involves assessing the public's perceived probability of a hazards and the probability of consequences (most often—negative consequences) (Bubeck et al. 2012 , Becker et al. 2013 , Grothmann and Reusswig 2006 ), is often equated with the awareness of the existence of a hazard and the resulting worry (Lechowska 2018 ).

Risk perception is a key social component in flood risk management (Bradford et al. 2012 ). Today, flood perception is recognized as a key factor in developing effective flood management strategies. How people perceive and understand flood risk affects the actions they take to prepare for and respond to flood events (Bradford et al. 2012 ). Therefore, in order to assess the usefulness of the applied research approaches for improving FRM, the study also included empirical works that, with reference to the level of flood risk perception, look for determinants of risk attitudes. For as Raaijmakers et al. ( 2008 ) flood risk perception can be considered as the relationship between the three specific risk characteristics: awareness, worry, and preparedness.

The issue addressed in this paper is all the more important because of the contemporary phenomenon of flood risk underestimation, which poses an important challenge for flood risk management. In order to improve FRM, it seems necessary to indicate the direction of research on flood risk perception, the results of which will be used for conscious and controlled moderation of perception and attitude toward flood risk, in effect developing a risk communication strategy to increase the resilience of the population to flood risks.

Motivated by many goals, orientations, and research interests, the studies of flood risk perception have presented a heterogeneous picture. Perception research has been conducted by researchers from various fields of science, resulting in a discrepancy in the basic theoretical issues (even the definition of risk), a different way of formulating a research problem, and the diversity of applied methods.

Bubeck et al. ( 2012 ) and Kellens et al. ( 2013 ) have stated, based on a literature review on the perception of flood risk, that there is no basic theoretical framework available within the social sciences. This work aims to fill this gap and strengthen the theoretical basis of the research on the perception of flood risk distinguishing on the grounds of epistemology two basic paradigms—rationalist and constructivist—similar to Brikholz et al. ( 2014 ). However, these paradigms are not dichotomous (absolute). Their distinction is aimed at highlighting heterogeneity in the field of flood risk perception research—discrepancies in applied approaches. The terms rationalist and constructivist have epistemic connotations, which cannot be simply characterized by methodological approaches that would be strictly pertinent to one or the other epistemology.

This paper aims to answer the main research question: research in which approach provides more useful information to improve flood risk management. Three specific questions were raised to meet the research objective:

Which paradigm dominates the research of flood risk perception?

What is the relationship between rationalistic and constructivistic factors (e.g., stimulation, weakening, strengthening, etc.)?

Which factors are more effective in moderating attitudes toward flood risk?

The relevance of rationalist and constructivist approaches in the study of flood risk perception and flood risk management more broadly was assessed through:

Explore, how the substantive scope of research on flood risk perception factors in the context of applied paradigms has evolved by proposing the categorization of the studied factors according to their character and applied research approach;

Determining the interrelationships between the various factors of a rationalist and constructivist nature (and between the paradigms more broadly) by comparing the results of research conducted in the two distinguished research streams;

Determining the role of rationalist and constructivist factors in shaping flood risk perception and attitudes toward flood risk.

The culmination of the purpose of this paper is to indicate the desired direction of flood risk perception research from the perspective of improving flood risk management.

An analysis of the results of the two strands of research will make it possible to identify factors of a rationalist and constructivist nature that unambiguously determine and undetermine the perception of flood risk and hence the attitude toward risk, as well as those with an ambiguous influence. For flood risk management the most important studies are those that give unambiguous results of the influence of a given factor on the phenomenon under study. Thus, those with inconsistent influence were considered effective factors for moderating perception and attitude toward flood risk. To strengthen their role in improving FRM, the possibility of their control by decision makers was also taken into account. Importantly, additionally, a cross-comparison of the results of studies conducted in these two paradigms provides information about the interplay of the influence of rationalist and constructivist factors.

Many studies have listed the factors that may influence flood risk assessment, but none considers the issue in the context of assessing the importance of rationalist and constructivist approaches to flood risk management. The work of Brikholz et al. ( 2014 ) refers to the aforementioned approaches but does not focus on the analysis of factors affecting flood risk assessment. This article therefore attempts to fill research gap in this area. Most of the reviews have focused on identifying predictors of adaptive behavior to mitigate flood risks (Bubeck et al. 2012 , Koerth et al. 2017 , Bamberg et al. 2017 , van Valkengoed and Steg 2019 , Kellens et al. 2013 ). Some have only considered one type of factor, e.g., flood experience, gender, education, and employment status as factors of flood risk perception understood as a combination of awareness, preparedness, and worry (Bradford et al. 2012 ), knowledge, trust, protection responsibility, physical exposure, hazard experience, and socio-demographics as variables affecting flood risk perception and preparedness—application of adaptive measures (Kellens et al. 2013 ), experience and trust in authorities and experts as factors influencing risk perception of natural hazards (including floods) and behavioral responses (Wachinger et al. 2013 ), or the spatial scope of the analyses is strictly defined, e.g., Central and Eastern European countries (Raška 2015 ). Few studies have compared the research results of risk perception in a transnational perspective (e.g., Boholm 1998 ).

The paper is divided into sections reflecting the logical sequence of the research. The introduction describes the significance of the issue being addressed and the purpose of the research. The second section is devoted to characterizing the rationalist and constructivist approaches. The next third presents the classifications of flood risk perception factors available in the literature. The fourth section describes the research method in detail, including the thought pattern and classifies the flood risk perception factors according to the research streams. The fifth part is devoted to the results of the research, more specifically, to presenting the evolution of the substantive scope of the factors of flood risk perception and to comparing the results of the research conducted in the two distinguished research streams. Conclusions and discussion answered the research questions, including identifying the interrelationships between the various rationalist and constructivist factors (and between paradigms more broadly), identifying the role of rationalist and constructivist factors in shaping attitudes toward flood risk, assessing the significance of rationalist and constructivist approaches in research on the perception of flood risk and more broadly in flood risk management. The paper concludes by indicating the desirable direction of flood risk perception research from the perspective of improving flood risk management.

2 Rationalist and constructivist paradigm

2.1 rationalist paradigm.

The rationalist approach to risk perception stresses individual cognitive processes and assumes that the existence of a threat induces an individual to make an assessment or judgment that feeds a "rational" decision-making process regarding the need to adopt a protective or preventive behavior (Birkholz et al. 2014 ). Studies rooted in the rationalist approach have tended to focus on modeling, characterizing, and predicting behavioral results regarding various threats. Such substantive approaches, for example, limited rationality and protection motivation theory, are largely rooted in the rationalist paradigm (closely related to cognitive psychology) and reveal the complex dependence of factors that can influence decision-making regarding protection against flood risk, including those that are contextual in nature (Birkholz et al. 2014 ) (Fig.  1 ). The rationalist paradigm assumes that individual risk preferences and behavioral results are the effects of a logical assessment of the likely outcomes, as well as related costs and benefits. Of interest is how individuals assess the costs and benefits of living in the affected areas (White 1945 ; Kates 1963 ; Burton et al. 1965 ).

figure 1

Theories in rationalistic and constructivistic paradigm. Source own elaboration based on Birkholz et al. ( 2014 ), Lichtenstein et al. ( 1978 ), Boholm ( 1998 ), Shaw et al. (2004), Grothmann and Reusswig ( 2006 ) and Zaalberg et al. ( 2009 ), Kuhlicke et al. ( 2020 )

In addition to cost–benefit evaluations conducted in the revealed preferences approach, another important approach in the rationalist paradigm is psychometric research (Birkholz et al. 2014 ) (Fig.  1 ). This theory has been introduced by Fischhoff et al. ( 1978 ) and Slovic ( 1987 ) and interprets risk as a cognitive construct that can be described psychometrically to reveal quantitative levels of evaluation (De Marchi 2007 ). The psychometric approach was initially developed by psychologists as a way of quantifying the individual perception of risk. It assumes that people interpret the world in terms of cause-and-effect patterns, and the existence of cognitive patterns allows modeling (by statistical methods) of a psychological phenomenon (Kellens et al. 2013 ). In psychometric studies, questionnaire surveys are the most often used source of data acquisition and help researchers search for cognitive variables that affect the perception of risk.

In the initial phase of psychometric research, researchers have focused on describing the qualitative characteristics of various types of risks (from environmental, through technological, to everyday risks such as communication), which were then replicated in different countries (Fischhoff et al. 1978 ; Slovic et al. 1980 ; Vlek Stallen 1981 ; Otway and Winterfeldt 1982 ; Englander et al. 1986 ; Teigen et al. 1988 ; Keown 1989 ; Goszczyńska et al. 1991 ; Karpowicz-Lazreg and Mullet 1993 ; Poumadere et al. 1995 ; Kleinhesselink and Rosa 1991 , 1994 ). Later, the qualitative features of risk characterization were eliminated in favor of studying the relationship between risk perception and socio-demographic predictors such as gender, age, education, income, or social status (e.g., Flynn et al. 1994 ; Siegrist and Gutscher 2008 ; Lindell and Hwang 2008 ; Oasim et al. 2015 ). Additionally, the group of examined explanatory variables has been gradually extended by others, for example, in the field of exposure to risk or disaster experiences (Kellens et al. 2011 ; Barnett and Breakwell 2001 ; Baan and Klijn 2004 ; Knocke and Kolivras 2007 ; Werritty et al. 2007 ). Subsequently, as part of psychometric research, attention has been focused on the study of adaptive behavior toward risk—to search for predictors of desired attitudes (Miceli et al. 2008 ; Grothmann and Reusswig 2006 ; Botzen et al. 2009a ; b).

2.2 Constructivist paradigm

The constructivist paradigm largely rejects the idea that the assessment of threats is an objective phenomenon, independent of the social system. Under this approach, flood risk perception is perceived as socially constructed and closely related to the dynamics of changes in the social system, for example, values, beliefs, culture, institutions, and organizations (Drake 1992 ; Oliver-Smith 1996 ; Tierney 1999 ; Weichselgartner 2001 ; Johnson et al. 2004 ). The constructivist paradigm promotes the concept of risk as a contextual phenomenon (Birkholtz et al. 2014 ); it assumes that individual's judgments and decision-making processes are shaped and limited by social environments (Kates 1963 ; Drake 1992 ; Tierney 1999 ; Slovic 2000 ). The constructivist paradigm therefore recommends an analysis of how the sociocultural context shapes a broader understanding of risk; it draws attention to the mechanisms by which risk perception can be disseminated and legitimized at a broader social level (Tierney 1999 ). The constructivist paradigm provides insights into factors that drive, transform, and moderate the dynamics between risk perception and management, for example organizational, institutional, and social factors (Gustafson 1998 ). For example, Tierney ( 1999 ) provides a penetrating picture of how institutions dictate to the public the understanding of risk and how social inequalities combine with exposure to risk.

One important theoretical current associated with constructivist thinking is cultural theory, which assumes that the perception and acceptance of risk are rooted in social and cultural norms (Shaw et al. 2004 ) (Fig.  1 ). Proponents of cultural theory suggest that it is possible to determine the probable perception of risk derived from cultural affiliation and social understanding. In this theory, attention is paid to how the structures of a social organization convey and strengthen the perception of risk by an individual (Douglas and Wildavsky 1982 ). The concept of risk in society according to the cultural theory is considered in a broader perspective, embracing moral values and considerations, as well as political, social, and ideological issues (see Douglas and Wildavsky 1982 ; Rayner and Cantor 1987 ; Thompson et al. 1990 ; Wildavsky and Dake 1990 ; Dake 1991 ; Douglas 1992 ). For example, Douglas and Wildavsky ( 1982 ) link four "worldview(s)" (individualistic, egalitarian, hierarchical, and fatalistic) with some attitudes toward risk. These "worldview(s)" proposed by Douglas ( 1978 ) are the expression of the application of cultural theory in risk perception studies because they reflect the combination of "cultural prejudices" (common values and beliefs) and "social relations" (models of interpersonal relations).

Two groups of sociological theories can be distinguished that fit into the research on attitudes to risk carried out in the constructivist paradigm: focusing on the interplay of society and individual practices (practice theories) and network theories (Fig.  1 ). According to the former, an individual's behavior is always embedded in social structures and the power relations associated with them. This behavior is modified in everyday social interactions and practices (Bourdieu 2010 ; Giddens 1979 ; Reckwitz 2002 , Ober and Sakdapolrak 2017 ). Network theories focus on social capital understood as "features of social organization, such as trust, norms and networks that can improve the efficiency of society by facilitating coordinated actions" (Putman 1993 , p. 167). These are theories with an emphasis on collective behavior. They include the social identity model of collective action (SIMCA) and social identity model of pro-environmental action (SIMPEA). SIMCA examines the impact of group efficacy beliefs in bringing about change and group-related anger on motivating people to band together to improve the group as a whole Van Zomeren et al. 2008 . SIMPEA shows how social identity processes influence judgment and behavior in the face of large-scale environmental crises. Social identity is the human ability to define oneself in terms of “We” rather than “I,” enabling people to think and act as a collective, which should be crucial given personal inadequacy in assessing and responding effectively to environmental crises (Fritsche et al. 2018 ).

The line of research conducted in both the rationalist and constructivist paradigms is "heuristics." In studies of flood risk perception, the heuristic of accessibility and the heuristic of affect have been considered the most (Fig.  1 ). The first one refers to what people remember and not what actually occurred (Lichtenstein et al. 1978 ). It is posited that people in conditions of uncertainty are guided by certain mental models that reflect the human thought process and refer to an individual’s assessment of the situation (Birkholtz et al. 2014 ). For example, threats that are more dramatic and spectacular can be more easily remembered, and their higher cognitive "availability" can explain the tendency among respondents to overestimate the risk of such threats (Lichtenstein et al. 1978 ). Papers in cognitive psychology have indicated that cognitive models and mental mechanisms of risk assessment are constantly moderated (modified, strengthened, or weakened) by media reports and influenced by friends, family, and various communication processes (Morgan et al. 2001 ). Research on risk perception (e.g., environmental, technological, road, and aviation hazards) has demonstrated the media's influence on the ease of recalling threats, resulting in an overstated risk assessment (Johnson and Tversky 1983 ). Additionally, Boholm ( 1998 ) emphasized that if the heuristic of accessibility is to be taken seriously as a theoretical framework for understanding risk perception, attention should be paid to how threats are represented socially, for example, in the media. The heuristic of affect thus says that mental representations are "marked" by weak negative or positive feelings. Finucane et al. ( 2000 ) showed that the perception of risk is an "affect marker"; namely, risk assessment often occurs under the influence of emotions. Siegrist and Gutscher ( 2008 ), Miceli et al. ( 2008 ), and Terpstra ( 2011 ) have also pointed out the importance of affect in perceiving and communicating the risk of flooding.

3 Classifications of flood risk perception factors in the literature

Many classifications of factors have been presented in the literature according to their nature (Table 1 ). Tobin and Montz ( 1997 ) distinguish situational and cognitive factors, and Wachinger et al. ( 2013 ) identify informational, personal, and contextual agents. Contextual factors have also been observed in Boholm ( 1998 ) and Brikholz et al. ( 2014 ). O’Neill et al. ( 2016 ) focus on cognitive (behavioral), socioeconomic, and geographical factors. A reasonably up-to-date classification of flood risk perception agents is in Lechowska ( 2018 ), in which six groups of factors (cognitive, behavioral, socioeconomic, demographic, geographical [physics], and informational and contextual) were identified. The featured classifications are inconsistent; they are developed at different levels of detail and present different perspectives on the interpretation of the nature of flood risk perception factors. Additionally, they are divided based on diverse criteria for the division. However, none of them considers the application of rationalist or constructivist paradigm. Therefore, it seems worthwhile to categorize the factors used in the research, based on approaches presented in the introduction of this paper, which was done in Sect. 4.3.

4 Research design

4.1 methods.

To achieve the goals set in the introduction, a research procedure was conducted comprising stages shown in Fig.  2 .

figure 2

Research design

The empirical works devoted to flood risk assessment research published in 1998–2018 to capture contemporary trends in research in this area were selected in the following manner: (a) the empirical works were selected from Google Scholar, the Web of Science/Knowledge, and Scopus databases meeting the following input criteria: full-text available/accessible, language used: English language, journals: papers published in peer-reviewed journals, by using a keyword combination, namely flood, factors, risk perception, flood risk, preparedness, worry, awareness, threat, risk underestimation, and denial; (b) in the selected articles, the following sections were reviewed in the following order—abstract, introduction, discussion, and conclusion—(c) which allowed for the development of an initial list of factors that may affect the perception of flood risk and attitudes toward risk; (d) if the aforementioned parts of the article focused on the aforementioned subject matter, this article was subject to a deeper review (i.e., entire text); and (e) further selection of research was made using the "snowballing" strategy (works cited in papers selected first according to steps b-d in three iterations were analyzed). As a result, the review includes 48 empirical works that focus on determining the level of perception of flood risk and identifying its possible factors. The catalog of variables adopted in the analyzed studies results from either the purpose of the research or an earlier literature review conducted by the authors of the analyzed works. The analyzed studies are "a sample" of all published empirical studies in analyzed period of time, which to some extent limits the inference. The paper provides a generalized view of the main linkages and relationships identified in the literature. Nonetheless, the results of the review provide useful insights about the findings of flood risk perception studies, capture some trends and regularities, and suggest possible directions for future research.

In the second stage, a categorization was conducted of the factors examined based on how they were applied in particular research approaches (rationalist and constructivist). The nature of the factor resulted from the research paradigm used in the study.

To determine the dynamics of changes in the substantive scope of research on flood risk perception factors, the changes in dynamics of occurrence frequency of the given groups of flood risk perception factors were analyzed in the third stage. For this purpose, a frequency ratio was used, which expresses the percentage share of consideration of a given factor in the total number of considerations of all factors in research articles in the year studied. This analysis helps to indicate, which paradigm dominates the research of flood risk perception.

R F —frequency ratio. x 1 —consideration of a given factor. x n —the total number of considerations of all factors in research articles in the year.

In the next stage, the comparison of results based on the adopted research approach was prepared to assess whether the test results of a given approach were unambiguous or mutually exclusive. Contradictory research results indicate their low applicability in flood risk management, as it is not possible to effectively moderate people's attitudes toward flood risk based on uncertain, often contradictory findings. Unambiguous research results are more useful and effective in this regard—they clearly identify the factors that shape flood risk perception.

As already mentioned, the distinction between rationalistic and constructivistic paradigms is not dichotomous. Research conducted in these paradigms can overlap, making it possible to determine the interrelationship between them by comparing the results of research conducted in a rationalistic and constructivistic approach.

The above-mentioned stages made it possible to achieve the main goals of the study. At the end, recommendations were formulated on what should be the direction of flood risk perception studies to improve flood risk management?

4.2 Categorization of flood risk perception factors based on the applied research approach

The flood risk factor perception was classified as rationalist or constructivist based on the method, in which the factor was included in the study, what role it played, and how it was interpreted by the particular researcher conducting the empirical study. Thus, the very way in which a particular researcher approached the research (formulating the research question) conditioned the categorization of factors, i.e., whether he or she viewed them as a cognitive variable, a predictor in modeling cognitive patterns, as an element of a cause–effect scheme of conduct in a "rational" decision process, or as a socially constructed element in a context’s meaning. However, the classes are not dichotomous, and some factors can be assigned to many categories (e.g., gender can be used as rationalist or be socially defined/constructed). For the purpose of this work, mixed factors were also assigned to one of the categories by considering how the factor is framed by the researcher himself.

As a result, the following classes of factors were included in the rationalist paradigm: individual’s physical location, flood characteristics, residence characteristics, size of consequences, range of impact, direct experience, socioeconomic and demographic profiles, and information (knowledge). Accordingly, to constructivist paradigm agents such as indirect experience, the cultural–historical, religious, and political contexts were assigned (Fig.  3 ).

figure 3

Categorization factors of flood risk perception and attitude toward risk.

5.1 Evolution of substantive scope of research on flood risk perception factors

In the study group of empirical papers published between 1998 and 2018, the greatest interest in the study of factors shaping the perception of flood risk was in 2008 and 2009. These works are mostly from northern Europe (the Netherlands, Denmark, England) and Taiwan and to a lesser extent from Western Europe (France, Switzerland, Northern Italy). In 2011 and 2015, we observe more interest from researchers from Germany and Belgium and then from the Czech Republic and Romania. Toward the end of the time period analyzed, there is a decline in the interest of researchers in flood risk perception (Fig.  4 ).

figure 4

Number of studied empirical works devoted to the factors of perception of flood risk published from 1998 to 2018

The empirical studies analyzed discuss factors that may influence the perception of flood risk (Table 2 ). These studies are mostly from the rationalist perspective because they concern groups of variables such as an individual's physical location (flood hazard level—flood zone), flood characteristics, size of consequences (the amount of potential flood losses), range of impact (direct or indirect hazard depending on the application of flood protection measures), direct experience, socioeconomic and demographic profiles, and information (knowledge). The frequency ratio for considering the aforementioned factors in the analyzed papers is 85.5%. Other factors, such as the indirect experiences, cultural–historical contexts, and religious and political contexts expressed the constructivist paradigm, are to a lesser extent considered in studies of flood risk perception (the frequency ratio for their inclusion is 14.5% (Fig.  5 ).

figure 5

Flood risk perception factors frequency ratio

The psychometric approach prevails in the collected research works in the rationalist paradigm. The authors of these papers are attempting to statistically describe the phenomenon in the area of social research. These papers present a quantitative analysis of individual risk perception, assuming the existence of cognitive patterns that allow the studied phenomena to be modeled (Fischhoff et al. 1978 ; De Marchi 2007 ; Kellens et al. 2013 ). Therefore, in the aforementioned papers, these factors appear mainly as independent variables included in the analyzing of the perception of flood risk.

In studies of the perception of flood risk conducted in the rationalist approach, the impact of direct flood experience on the level of risk perception is considered (the frequency ratio is 25.9%), followed by social, economic, and demographic factors (23.2%; Fig.  4 ). Other factors less frequently considered in the studies of flood risk perception are knowledge level (15.2%), residence characteristics (10.7%), and individual's physical location (10.7%). Other groups of factors, such as the size of consequences, flood characteristics, and the range of impact, are considered to a marginal extent (frequency ratio below 10.0%).

In the works conducted using the constructivist paradigm, the political context is most often considered (frequency ratio is 47.4%), followed by indirect experience (31.6%; Fig.  4 ). Cultural–historical and religious contexts are included to a lesser extent (frequency ratio of 15.8 and 5.3%, respectively).

Next, the development of researchers' interest in particular groups of factors that may influence the perception of flood risk is examined. At the beginning of the study period, the focus was on rationalistic factors. From 1998 to 2001, there was great interest in determining the influence of such factors as socioeconomic and demographic profiles, the range of impact and information (knowledge) on the level of flood risk perception. From 2002 to 2007, the focus is on direct experience and the related size of consequences, as well as residence characteristics. From 2008 to 2018, researchers focus on factors such as direct experience, socioeconomic and demographic profiles, information (knowledge), and residence characteristics, as well as an individual's physical location (Fig.  6 ).

figure 6

Number of studied empirical papers by included factors of flood risk perception published from 1998 to 2018

Regarding constructivist factors (indirect experience and political, cultural–historical, and religious contexts), they began to be considered in empirical works on the perception of flood risk in 2009, later than the rationalist factors. From 1998 to 2003, research on the perception of flood risk primarily focuses on the religious context and interpersonal communication networks (shaping intermediate experience). From 2004 to 2012, the primary focus is political context and, from 2013 to 2018, the cultural and historical contexts (Fig.  6 ).

5.2 Impact of individual factors on the flood risk perception and attitude toward risk

Flood risk perception in empirical studies is expressed by worry and awareness and is often identified with it. A separate issue in the flood risk perception research is preparedness, defining attitude toward risk, and search for factors that would influence taking preventive measures in relation to the level of flood risk perception (Lechowska 2018 ). Therefore, the influence of factors on flood risk perception (including awareness and worry) and on attitudes toward risk is discussed separately.

5.2.1 Rationalist paradigm

The most attention is paid to factors that affect the perception of risk, followed by those factors that have no unequivocal influence on the studied phenomenon, and then the group of problematic factors (affecting perception).

Empirical research demonstrates that the transfer of information and the education of the public increase awareness (King 2000 ; Raaijmakers et al. 2008 ), similar to the experience of flooding (Lindell and Hwang 2008 ) and the very fact of the flood (Biernacki et al. 2009 ). Studies have also shown that people with a lower education are more worried about floods than those with a higher education are (Sjöberg 1998 ; Bradford et al. 2012 ). Bradford et al. ( 2012 ) assume that there is a relationship between education and income level and conclude that individuals with higher incomes are less worried about the consequences of flooding than those with lower incomes are (Fig.  7 ).

figure 7

Impact of factors on flood risk perception and its three aspects—a comparison of the results of a rationalist approach

Empirical studies demonstrate the impact of knowledge on the perception of risk. People with little knowledge have a lower risk perception (Messner and Meyer 2006 ; Raaijmakers et al. 2008 ; Botzen et al. 2009a ; Biernacki et al. 2009 ; Działek et al. 2013 ; Wachinger et al. 2013 ). Many studies have emphasized the significance of an individual’s experience in shaping the perception of flood risk (Barnett and Breakwell 2001 ; Slovic et al. 2004 ; Kreibich et al. 2005 ; Grothmann and Reusswig 2006 ; Siegrist and Gutscher 2008 ; Zaalberg et al. 2009 ; Botzen et al. 2009a ; Kellens et al. 2011 ; Duží et al. 2014; Bustillos Ardaya et al. 2017 ). People who have been directly affected by floods are characterized by a higher level of perception of flood risk (Kellens et al. 2011 ; Oasim et al. 2015 ; Thistlethwaite et al. 2018 ; Fig.  7 ).

Another factor that influences the perception of flood risk is gender. Kellens et al. ( 2011 ), Bustillos and Ardaya et al. ( 2017 ), and Lindell and Hwang ( 2008 ) has unequivocally indicated that women usually perceive a higher level of flood risk (Fig.  7 ).

In the research in the rationalist trend, researchers have demonstrated the influence of particular factors on the perception of flood risk but often excluded their impact, i.e., it shows the lack of effect of the variable on the phenomenon under study (Fig.  7 ).

Socioeconomic features such as income, age, gender, and education do not affect awareness. It has also been shown that neither income (Botzen et al. 2009a ; Oasim et al. 2015 ) nor household size (Takao et al. 2004 ; Zaalberg et al. 2009 ; Oasim et al. 2015 ) have a significant impact on the perception of flood risk. Residence characteristics such as having a basement or living on the ground floor do not affect the level of perception of flood risk, as Kellens et al. ( 2011 ; Fig.  7 ) demonstrate.

A group of factors whose impact on the perception of flood risk is ambiguous is problematic. Bradford et al. ( 2012 ) do not show a strong relationship between anxiety and gender, whereas according to Sjöberg ( 1998 ) and Poorting et al. ( 2011 ) women more often show anxiety in response to perceived threats.

Kellens et al. ( 2011 ) show that older people usually assess the risk of flooding as higher than younger people, but according to Oasim et al. ( 2015 ) and Armas et al. ( 2015 ), age has no significant impact on the perception of flood risk. The level of education, according to Kellens et al. ( 2011 ) and Armas et al. ( 2015 ), is not decisive in risk perception; Oasim et al. ( 2015 ) and Botzen et al. ( 2009a ) prove that the level of education increases the perception of flood risk (Fig.  8 ).

figure 8

Impact of particular factors on the perception of flood risk and on three aspects of flood risk perception—a summary of the results of a rationalist approach

The analyzed literature suggests that real estate ownership is associated with a higher level of perception of flood risk than apartment rental (Grothmann and Reusswig 2006 ; Burningham et al. 2008 ; Oasim et al. 2015 ), and Kellens et al. ( 2011 ) show no such a relationship (Fig.  8 ).

This discrepancy in the results also applies to the impact of the proximity of the hazard (river) on the level of perceived risk. According to some studies, the proximity of the source of hazard results in a higher perception of flood risk (Lindell and Hwang 2008 ; Miceli et al. 2008 ; Zhang et al. 2010 ; O’Neill et al. 2016 ), and others posit that the closer the river, the lower the perception of risk, due to the incidental nature of flooding (Oasim et al. 2015 ; Colten and Sumpter 2009 ). Kellens et al. ( 2011 ) obtain no dependencies in this respect (Fig.  8 ).

The impact of the duration of residence on the perception of flood risk is also unclear. Thistlethwaite et al. ( 2018 ) demonstrate a moderate positive relationship, but Bustillos and Ardaya et al. ( 2017 ) demonstrate a very weak one (Fig.  8 ).

Concerning the factors that shape attitudes toward risk, the largest group are factors that have ambiguous effects on taking countermeasures. There is no match between the results of research on the impact of experience and knowledge (information) on the level of preparedness. Positive dependence between the experience of flood and the degree of involvement in the preparation of individual mitigation measures has been demonstrated by Takao et al. ( 2004 ), Slovic et al. ( 2004 ), Grothmann and Reusswig ( 2006 ), Siegrist and Gutscher ( 2008 ), Lamond et al. ( 2009 ), Biernacki et al. ( 2009 ), Terpstra ( 2011 ), Kreibich et al. ( 2011 ), Stojanow et al. ( 2015 ), and Thistlethwaite et al. ( 2018 ), and the correlation and regression values obtained have often been low or average (Grothmann and Reusswig 2006 ; Thieken et al. 2007 ; Siegrist and Gutscher 2008 ; Lindell and Hwang 2008 ; Miceli et al. 2008 ). By contrast, Takao et al. ( 2004 ) and Thieken et al. ( 2007 ) and Duží et al. ( 2015 ) do not show such dependence. The level of knowledge of floods, according to Thieken et al. ( 2007 ), Miceli et al. ( 2008 ), weakly correlates positively with preventive actions, and Botzen et al. ( 2009a ) show that the increase in knowledge of a threat reduces the willingness of the population to invest in protective measures; however, Siegrist and Gutscher ( 2008 ) show no such dependence (Fig.  8 ).

The results of the analyzed studies also ambiguously specify the relationship between the location in flood hazard zones and undertaking preventive actions. According to Duží et al. ( 2015 ), with an increase in hazard level, risk mitigation measures are more often used. Kunreuther ( 1996 ), by contrast, notes that people living in the most vulnerable areas rarely voluntarily take precautionary measures, increasing their vulnerability to disasters. In a study by Bera and Daněk ( 2018 ), the reluctance to subsidize in flood protection measures is higher the more threatened the village is (Fig.  8 ).

Age and income are the factors that have an ambiguous effect on readiness in the analyzed papers. According to some researchers, the age of inhabitants of flood zones has little or no significance in undertaking individual protective actions (Knocke and Kolivras 2007 ; Lindell and Hwang 2008 ; Zaalberg et al. 2009 ; Botzen et al. 2009a ), while Działek et al., 2013 ), Grothmann and Reusswig ( 2006 ) show that older residents of these areas more often decide to use preventive measures. The significance of income for floodplain inhabitants taking preventive measures is low, according Lindell and Hwang ( 2008 ), Botzen et al. ( 2009a ), and Zaalberg et al. ( 2009 ), but according to Grothmann and Reusswig ( 2006 ), Stojanow et al. ( 2015 ), and Thistlethwaite et al. ( 2018 ) more affluent residents are more willing to invest in flood mitigation measures (Fig.  8 ).

In contrast, a relationship is demonstrated between gender and having children and preparedness: men and households with children are more likely to take preventive measures (Stojanov et al. 2015 ; Fitton et al. 2015 ). In addition, Grothmann and Reusswig ( 2006 ), Harries and Penning-Rowsell ( 2011 ), and Thistlethwaite et al. ( 2018 ) have demonstrated a positive impact of owning a home on taking mitigation measures. For preventive behaviors, the type of building is also important. Applying security measures is much lower for multi-family buildings than for single-family buildings (Działek et al. 2013 , 2014 ; Biernacki et al. 2009 ).

Many studies have also demonstrated very little or no effect of education on precaution (Sjöberg 1998 ; Grothmann and Reusswig 2006 ; Lindell and Hwang 2008 ; Zaalberg et al. 2009 ; Botzen et al. 2009a ). In addition, the distance from the threat (river) seems to have little effect on mitigating behaviors (Lindell and Hwang 2008 ; Miceli et al. 2008 ; Botzen et al. 2009a ).

5.2.2 Constructivist paradigm

Research conducted using the constructivist approach indicates the great importance of cultural values, religion, political system, history, and types of social bonds in shaping the perception of flood risk (Fig.  9 ).

figure 9

Influence of factors on the perception of flood risk and attitude toward risk—a summary of the results of the constructivist approach

In studies that consider the social context, the impact of the communication network in the form of media (Biernacki et al. 2009 ) and personal interactions with other people on the assessment of flood risk are considered. Działek et al. ( 2013 , 2014 ) demonstrate that the ability to obtain information and recall the experience depends largely on the type of social structures and social capital. In rural communities and small towns characterized by strong social bonds (bonding social capital), the importance of disseminating information on local hazards between generations by the inhabitants is observed. However, in the case of communities with weak social ties (bridging social capital), such as large cities, indirect experiences of residents are difficult to convey—populations are more mobile and personal interactions between people are limited (Działek et al. 2013 , 2014 ; Cutter et al. 2003 ). According to Terpstra et al. ( 2009 ), other people's experiences and reports communicated through social communication—indirect experiences (e.g., the effects of floods experienced by friends, relatives, neighbors, and other people)—have a smaller impact on risk perception than personal, direct experience. Media coverage contributes to the perception of risk mainly when the recipient has no flood-related personal, direct experience (Wachinger et al. 2013 ), and according to Bustillos Ardaya et al. ( 2017 ), information provided at the local level (from neighbors, family, and friends) is more important in shaping risk perception than deliberate action by institutions through various information policies are.

Because few papers in the study group consider the social context in the research on risk perception, we refer to the results of the research with a more general character, namely those that refer to all hazards (technological, social, natural). Some results have demonstrated (e.g., Johnson and Tversky 1983 ; Short 1984 ) that media coverage may be biased, which strengthens the dramatism of catastrophic events while weakening the importance of everyday threats. Sjöberg et al. ( 1996 ) aptly note that the role of media in risk perception is complex and differs by society. For example, media in communist countries poorly inform residents about hazards and understate risk perception because they are more closely controlled by the government than in democratic states (Englander et al. 1986 ). Thus, the role of the next constructivist factor, namely the political and economic system that diversifies the role of the media in society, is emphasized.

In addition to what was aforementioned, the political context is considered by defining the role of public authorities in shaping the perception of flood risk, including defining the confidence in authorities and public flood protection measures and the associated sense of self-responsibility for risk mitigation activities. The conviction that the authorities bear the main responsibility for flood protection thus removes this burden from residents and is confirmed by Bichard and Kaźmierczak ( 2012 ) and by Terpstra and Gutteling ( 2008 ). As shown by Bera and Daněk ( 2018 ), reliance on public flood protection has resulted in not taking flood control measures . Similarly, Hung ( 2009 ) and Terpstra ( 2011 ) state that the confidence in public flood protection decreases with the increase in residents' preparedness and willingness to acquire insurance, and according to Bubeck et al. ( 2012 ), the actions of the authorities can be an obstacle to taking private mitigation measures. One such example is protection in the form of levees, which provides an apparent sense of security (the so-called levee effect) to the inhabitants of flood plains. This perception results in the underestimation of flood risk and inhibition of activities toward proper protection against floods (Burton and Cutter 2008 ; Terpstra 2009 ; Kousky and Kunreuther 2009 ; Ludy and Kondolf 2012 ). Notably, trust in the authorities becomes important in the case of low individual knowledge of the threat (Kellens et al. 2013 ; Wachinger et al. 2013 ). Trust in flood protection measures (the belief in their effectiveness) often results from previous flood experiences (Wachinger et al. 2013 ; Birkholtz et al. 2014 ).

The cultural context in the study of flood risk perception is considered by Armas et al. ( 2015 ), who conduct ethnographic research. Cultural conditions of the studied community constitute the background and complementation of the interpretation of the obtained psychometric results. With a small number of empirical studies on the cultural context published from 2008 to 2018, we must mention the earlier works on cultural values in risk assessment in relation to various types of threats. For example, Rohmann ( 1994 ) shows that risk assessments differ significantly in orientation (technological, monetary, environmental, and feminist). In other studies, the phenomena of “cross-cultural, cross-gender reversal” or the dependence of the impact of gender on the level of risk perception are considered . The assessment of risk perception of women in Japan and men in the United States was similar (Kleinhesselink and Rosa 1991 ). Gender as a variable plays a greater role in countries with more marked legal and cultural differences between the genders than those that do not. Also Gustafson ( 1998 ) with gender theory tried to explain the different perception of flood risk by men and women.

The historical element (apart from the direct experience of flooding) in contemporary studies of flood risk perception is explored in Działek et al. ( 2013 , 2014 ), who underline the impact of the historical paths of the studied area on the strength of social bonds and type of social capital, and hence the indirect experience, on flood risk perception.

The problem of the influence of religion on the perception and attitudes toward flood risk was analyzed by Schmuck ( 2000 ) in the Muslim community, confirming the existence of this dependence in the studied group. In the analyzed community, the conviction that the flood is the work of Allah led to abandoning all actions aimed at the preparedness and mitigation of flood risks. Similarly, in Bustillos Ardaya et al. ( 2017 ), Brazilian respondents, when asked why private precautions were not taken, answered: "Whatever God's will is, it will happen," "whenever He wants to take me, it is because it is my time," or "it is not possible to know the intentions of the Lord." (p. 235). In this context, the importance of fatalism, wishful thinking, and hopelessness related to religion in shaping attitudes toward risk is observed. Fatalism and denial are among the non-protective reactions resulting from an inadequate assessment of handling the risk (e.g., response efficacy, self-efficacy, and response costs) with a simultaneous high-risk assessment. This approach to risk concerns people who, through their experience of flooding, are convinced of their inability to control such events. Fatalism, wishful thinking, and denial prevent only the negative emotional consequences of perceived risk, such as fear (Bustillos Ardaya et al. 2017 ). Notably, the religious and cultural context is also important in the selection of the studied characteristics of the population. For example, in the case of Muslim communities, women are excluded from the study (Oasim et al. 2015 ).

6 Discussion and conclusions

The abundance of research concerning the level of flood risk perception makes it a dynamic field of empirical research, the resources of which should interest decision makers managing flood risk. The empirical works analyzed provide a broad view of the applied research approaches.

Rationalists focus on the quantitative approach, probability, and consequences, and constructivists are more interested in the qualitative dimension. The constructivist approach may be criticized by researchers for its qualitative character and difficulty in operationalization (Oasim et al. 2014).

The topics and research interests concerning factors or groups of factors that may influence the perception of flood risk varied over time. In the beginning, attention was paid to the rationalistic approach (mainly to direct experience, socioeconomic and demographic profiles, and information, and later also to residence characteristics and individual's physical location). Over time, the amount of constructivist research increased, particularly that related to indirect experience and a political context. Research interests and their variations over time could result from the availability of data and development of research tools. For example, it is much easier to obtain (and measure) metric information on respondents (e.g., age, gender, education) than their more intimate (personal) data, such as religion and relationships with family or neighbors, and that on historical and cultural backgrounds. With the development of geographical information systems, the scope, detail, and availability of data increased, including techniques for delimiting flood zones, flood characteristics, identification of an individual's physical location, and residence characteristics.

The choice of factors for the perception of flood risk is often simply a matter of interest of an individual researcher. It may also be determined by the research results of other literature due to the easiness of the later discussion on the results obtained. This reasoning can sometimes become a mechanism of a "self-winding circle," resulting in the excessive expansion of one sphere of research at the expense of another.

Detailed conclusions of the review are presented in the form of answers to the research questions posed in the introduction.

6.1 Which paradigm dominates the research of flood risk perception?

Contemporary empirical works on flood risk perception are dominated by a rationalistic approach that psychometrically searches for cognitive patterns of behaviors when there is flood risk; however, research in the rationalist approach creates a heterogeneous field of research in terms of basic theoretical issues, methodology, and more importantly, the results obtained, as also highlighted in his review by Andráško ( 2021 ) in relation to the study of factors influencing attitudes toward risk in taking adaptive action. Often, statistically obtained dependencies are mutually exclusive. Physical, cognitive, behavioral, and demographic–economic variables alone cannot adequately explain attitudes toward flood risk. One limitation of some rationalistic studies is the "flattening" of variables or the inability to consider them because of external factors. For example, in the case of a location variable, there was a high degree of homogeneity (low variance), which resulted in lowering the importance of this variable in the results.

Thus, studies on flood risk perception in the constructivist approach are at an early stage of development. Currently, they are only a supplement to rationalistic research and not a mainstream tendency in the field of flood risk perception. However, studies of flood risk assessment by the public in a social, political, cultural, religious, or historical context provide a clear results.

6.2 What is the relationship between rationalistic and constructivistic factors (e.g., stimulation, weakening, strengthening, etc.)?

The analysis of the collected empirical works indicates mutual relations between individual factors of a rationalistic and constructivist nature. Andráško ( 2021 ) is of the opinion that the factors influencing risk attitudes are interrelated in some kind of complex relationship, but it is not entirely clear how the various elements interact, what is the nature of their (causal) connections, or what kinds of outcomes they produce.

This review has demonstrated that the influence of rationalistic factors on the level of flood risk perception often results from the constructivist context. For example, media coverage plays a greater role in shaping risk perceptions when the recipient does not have personal direct experience of flooding (Wachinger et al. 2013 ), and other people's experiences (e.g., the effects of the flood experienced by friends, relatives, neighbors, and other people) have a weaker impact on the perception of risk than personal direct experience (Terpstra et al. 2009 ). Trust in the authorities becomes more important when there is little individual knowledge of risk, and trust in flood protection measures (confidence in their effectiveness) often results from the previous experience of flooding. Furthermore, gender plays a greater role in shaping the perception of flood risk in countries where legal and cultural differences between the sexes are more outlined.

Constructivist research points to the moderating role of context in studies of flood risk perception. Constructivist factors weaken or strengthen the impact of rationalistic factors on the level of flood risk perception in general and attitude to risk. For example, negative feelings related to previous experiences decrease trust in authorities and official flood protection measures, while positive feelings increase trust. High trust public protection measures reinforce the influence of personal flood experience on risk perception and vice versa. Factors that modify the influence of knowledge on risk perception are communication networks in the form of media and personal interaction with other people (indirect experience) and the type of social capital and social ties, e.g., close social ties weaken the stimulating effect of knowledge on risk perception and vice versa. The policy of the authorities in providing public flood protection due to historical circumstances and the level of trust of the population in the authorities and in public protective measures moderates the relationship between risk perception and preparedness, e.g., the tradition of centralized policy in post-communist countries resulting in public reliance on public flood protection weakens the positive impact of risk perception on taking countermeasures. The relationship between perception and preparedness is also weakened by religion, e.g., in Muslim communities. Gender as a determinant variable of flood risk perception plays a greater role in countries with more pronounced legal and cultural gender differences ('cross-cultural, cross-gender reversal'). A constructivist approach to studying flood risk perception can deepen the understanding of the various, often antagonistic, results of rationalistic research.

Contemporary literature reviews are beginning to point out, that the influence of rationalistic factors on attitudes toward flood risk is driven by the influence of contextual factors. According to Andráško ( 2021 ), the roles assigned to individuals and social relationships/ ties/norms/structures are often the decisive factors behind differences in empirical findings. He identifies a discrepancy in results regarding the role of socio-demographic characteristics in shaping flood risk behavior in different situations across communities. In his view, socio-demographic characteristics do not act as direct predictors or determinants but should be seen as mediators or enhancers of the main links between experience, perception, and preparedness. Fox-Roger et al. ( 2016 ) consider emotions, which are the mechanism that directs basic psychological processes, and their associated feelings as elements that can explain the disconnect between consciousness and readiness.

6.3 Which factors are more effective in moderating attitudes toward flood risk?

As the influence of rationalistic factors on the level of flood risk perception often results from the constructivist context, knowing specific local political, cultural, historical, religious, and social conditions makes it possible to more effectively moderate the attitudes of the inhabitants toward flood risks.

It seems that some constructivist factors, such as the influence of media or neighbors, friends, and family on flood risk perception, are more controllable than rationalistic factors (e.g., direct experience of flooding, or gender or age). Decision makers tend to be more influential in controlling constructivist than rationalist factors. It seems that more control can be had over the former (soft—less tangible) than the latter (hard) factors. Constructive studies therefore provide more useful information for improving flood risk management.

Contemporary literature reviews point to similar findings. For example, Andráško ( 2021 ) noted that the strength and quality of social networks are important factors influencing the ability of communities to cope with flood risk, including social position (such as class), sociopolitical organization, poverty, social inequality, and environmental injustice. Andráško ( 2021 ) points out that the way local communities function, interpersonal ties and networks, mutual support and assistance, the behavior and example of neighbors and friends, possible deterioration of relationships related to perceived unfair distribution of financial compensation, rumors and feelings of jealousy or reproach, as well as trust in the effective functioning of risk management determines in some way people's preparedness for flood events. Additionally, according to Andráško ( 2021 ), affect heuristics appear to be an important determinant of how people prepare for and respond to risks. Another research review (Kuhlicke et al. 2020 ) notes that factors such as local discourses and narratives, (perceived) injustice and marginalization of certain actors or entire regions, and social ties may influence adaptive behavior in relation to flood events. According to Kuhlicke et al. ( 2020 , p. 12) “ capacities to take adaptive actions are enmeshed in specific contexts.”

Notably, the development of technology has changed the sources of information about threats, which should be reflected in the research on risk perception. The level of knowledge of risk should not be determined solely based on the knowledge of flood hazard maps, but also, and perhaps primarily, on the basis of media coverage. Empirical studies show that the media influence the perception of flood risk (inter alia Biernacki et al. 2009 ).

Therefore, in the practice of flood risk management, particular attention should be paid to the role of social media in shaping attitudes toward risk (com. Reutera et al. 2019 ; Yuan et al.. 2021 ; Kim and Madison 2020 ). More dramatic and catastrophic events are more frequently covered in the media than everyday hazards. This results in everyday hazards being perceived as less dangerous, less frightening, and easier to deal with, leading to an underestimation of the risks associated with more frequent events (Short 1984 ). As aptly noted by Boholm ( 1998 ), the availability heuristic should be taken seriously as a theoretical framework for understanding risk perception. Attention should be paid to how risks are represented socially, for example in the media. Brikholz et al. ( 2014 ) are also of the opinion that the media can play a key role in the social construction of risk perception. Andráško ( 2021 ) suggests that media analysis involves examining the way in which the disaster is portrayed in the media, how it is reported, and at the same time analyzes the emotional ruminations of the audience of these presentations. In the era of a digital society, electronic media might be more effective in communicating risk. Neighborly ties, by contrast, are loosening up and will be of minor importance in determining the role of indirect experience in shaping attitudes toward risk. It is worth noting the potential of using artificial intelligence (AI) to study the perception of flood risk based on social data. Since it is possible to profile people based on their social network entries, it is also possible to find some common characteristics for groups of people who are more resistant or vulnerable to flood risk based on such networks.

Moreover, within the group of constructivist factors, some relations can be observed. Information provided at the local level (from neighbors, family, and friends) is more important in developing the risk perception than the institutional impact is. The political and economic system makes the role of the media in society different. (Greater control of the authorities results in a more limited amount information on risks.) Different historical backgrounds influence the power of social bonds and the type of social capital, shaping the role of indirect experiences in flood risk perception.

6.4 Research in which approach provides more useful information to improve flood risk management?

Consequently, flood risk perception research conducted in a constructivist paradigm provides more useful information in shaping attitudes toward risk and finally provides more useful information to improve flood risk management.

7 Recommendation

The research on flood risk perception should be reoriented toward studying the role of contextual factors in shaping risk perception. It is necessary to assess how the public interprets flood risks socially and culturally, and not merely in terms of metric data. The study of risk perception is a social study because it concerns the observation and analysis of processes and phenomena occurring in society and uses information about the needs and opinions of the population and social practices. Thus, all the elements that determine the nature of the society under study (its world view, cultural history, social structures, norms, power relations, etc.) should be considered. According to ethnographers, human social existence is culturally variable. Perceptions of events and phenomena are conditioned by values that differ locally. Brikholz et al. ( 2014 ) express the same opinion and agree with the need to comprehensively reinvigorate flood risk research, supported by a more constructive approach to flood risk management. Tierney ( 1999 ) argues for the use of a constructivist approach by saying that it provides mechanisms by which specific risk perception can be disseminated and legitimized at a wider social level.

The results of this empirical study show a slow shift toward research on soft flood risk perception factors, conducted in the constructivist approach. This is because the role of soft factors in shaping flood risk perception is increasingly important. The importance of the context in the research on the perception of flood risk has slowly begun to be recognized also in the reviews. For example, Raška ( 2015 ) perceives the causes of differences in the perception of environmental hazards (e.g., flood risk) occurring between residents of Western Europe and Central and Eastern Europe in different political and cultural contexts (historically conditioned). The post-communist political system (once ensuring a strong position of central government bodies) of the countries of Central and Eastern Europe shape the current passive attitude of the communities of these countries toward floods. In societies of Central and Eastern Europe, decentralized and liberalized as a result of systemic transformation, prevailing materialistic orientation causes greater anxiety and awareness of environmental threats than for the citizens of Western European countries with a post-materialist orientation. Moreover, Boholm ( 1998 ) reviews the results of international research draws attention to their diversity, which may result from different geographical, national, and cultural contexts of the studied countries.

In their review, Kuhlicke et al. ( 2020 ) highlight the need to embed FRM and its practice within larger sociopolitical structures. In formulating future research directions, they note the need to answer the question " how the respective societal context produces specific patterns of vulnerability with respect to flood events? " (Kuhlicke et al. 2020 , p. 13). They are of the opinion that researchers need to be sensitized to social processes and structures that can help explain specific patterns of vulnerability. According to them " there is (…) a need to develop a more nuanced understanding of which contextual factors shape households capacities. However, to allow for comparison of empirical insights generated by studies conducted in different contextual settings, we argue that a stronger theoretical underpinning of research in this intellectual catchment is needed to be able to produce empirical evidences that are comparable across different contexts " (Kuhlicke et al. 2020 , p. 12). As they aptly note, Comparative research could contrast and confirm results for different adaptation measures or different contexts. Attems et al. ( 2020 ) noted that, in fact, the influence of factors on individual behavior toward risk is quite complex, as different sociocultural and individual circumstances influence the individual behavior of individuals. Furthermore, according to Andráško ( 2021 ), the interrelationship between flooding and social processes is receiving increasing attention in flood risk management research. The role of the social environment is so far an under-researched area of flood risk research. Many social or sociopsychological theories and approaches are still not used to study perceptions and behaviors related to flooding. Andráško ( 2021 ) sees a need for further research into the (nature of) the relationship of socio-demographic characteristics with other factors of flood risk behavior, and a research gap regarding the study of the role of emotional ties and local identities in shaping risk behavior. As he aptly postulates, flood risk research should aim not only to identify factors, but also to determine how they are interrelated.

However, complete disregard for the rationalist approach is not possible in studies of flood risk perception. It should not be replaced by research in the constructivist paradigm but only constantly supplemented. Notably, the constructivist and rationalist perspectives are insufficiently reliable and credible. As Renn ( 1989 ) aptly points out, they are unable to provide solid guidance on their own, and public values, rational decision-making, and scientific knowledge should be reconciled through a “well-managed discourse.”

In connection with the above, there is a need to revive the study of flood risk perception toward a more holistic view, namely the development of a constructivist approach to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the factors affecting the perception of flood risk. The constructivist paradigm therefore recommends a more comprehensive analysis of how the sociocultural context shapes a broader understanding of risk. Research teams should include geographers and scientists representing other scientific disciplines, such as psychology, sociology, cultural studies, religious studies, ethnography, political science, history, social communication, and media.

Flood risk research has great potential to combine natural and social sciences. However, the fusion of theories derived from these two research areas is extremely difficult and caution must be exercised. The ontological, epistemological, axiological, and ethical foundations of these theories are often fundamentally different, with the result that the individual theories are not necessarily mutually compatible.

The postulated direction of flood risk research is part of the research concepts of vulnerability, capacity, and resilience. It closely refers to the innovative approach to developing a social resilience model, which would be based, among other things, on the perception of flood risk (Bradford et al. 2012 ). The concept of resilience assumes a positive adaptation of society to the existing developmental threats. A resilient society is one that adapts continuously and flexibly to changing circumstances. Community resilience has been highlighted as one of four priority mechanisms for disaster risk reduction worldwide (Schelfaut et al. 2011 ). The role of risk perception in improving the resilience of people and communities is widely recognized as an important part of the wider area of risk research (Burns and Slovic 2012 ). Risk perception is often viewed as a key element of vulnerability assessment.

In many studies, the perception of risk is considered a factor of vulnerability (e.g., Messner and Meyer 2006 ; Kuhlicke et al. 2011 ), but we can agree with Brikholz et al. ( 2014 ) that they rarely elaborate on the perception of flood risk in a substantive manner. Some resilience studies often consider the important role of risk perception as an argument for better risk communication in flood management strategies (e.g., Schelfaut et al. 2011 ). However, based on the literature on flood risk management, the perception of Kuhlicke and Steinführer ( 2013 ) is that the concept of resilience is largely in its infancy, and we agree. At present, relatively few empirical studies have examined the perception of flood risk in terms of resilience. The statement in Brikholtz et al. ( 2014 , p. 15) that the concept of resilience "should, we argue, be used to greater effect in underpinning flood risk research" exactly corresponds to the recommendations made in this work regarding a renewed research agenda for flood risk perception.

Andráško I (2021) Why people (do not) adopt the private precautionary and mitigation measures: a review of the issue from the perspective of recent flood risk research. Water 13(140)

Armas I, Ionescu R, Posner CN (2015) Flood risk perception along the Lower Danube river, Romania. Nat Hazards 79:1913–1931

Google Scholar  

Attems M-S, Thaler T, Genovese E, Fuchs S (2020) Implementation of property-level flood risk adaptation (PLFRA) measures: choices and decisions. WIREs Water 7(e1404)

Baan PJA, Klijn F (2004) Flood risk perception and implications for flood risk management in the Netherlands. Int J River Basin Manag 2(2):1–10

Bamberg S, Masson T, Brewitt K, Nemetschek N (2017) Threat, coping and flood prevention—A meta-analysis. J Environ Psychol 54:116–126

Barnett J, Breakwell GM (2001) Risk perception and experience: hazard personality profiles and individual differences. Risk Anal 21(1):171–177

Becker G, Aerts JCJH, Huitema D (2013) Influence of flood risk perception and other factors on risk-reducing behaviour: a survey of municipalities along the Rhine. J Flood Risk Manag 7(1):16–30

Bera MK, Daněk P (2018) The perception of risk in the flood-prone area: a case study from the Czech municipality. Disast Prev Manag 27(1):2–14

Bichard E, Kazmierczak A (2012) Are homeowners willing to adapt to and mitigate the effects of climate change? Clim Chang 112:633–654

Biernacki W, Bokwa W, Działek J, Padło T (2009) Społeczności lokalne wobec zagrożeń przyrodniczych i klęsk żywiołowych. Instytut Geografii i Gospodarki Przestrzennej Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego Kraków

Birkholz S, Muro M, Jeffrey P, Smith HM (2014) Rethinking the relationship between flood risk perception and flood management. Sci Total Environ 478:12–20

Boholm A (1998) Comparative studies of risk perception: a review of twenty years of research. J Risk Res 1(2):135–163

Botzen WJW, van den Bergh JCJM (2012) Monetary valuation of insurance against flood risk under climate change. Int Econ Rev 53(3):1005–1026

Botzen WJW, Aerts JCJH, van den Bergh JCJM (2009a) Willingness of homeowners to mitigate climate risk through insurance. Ecol Econ 68:2265–2277

Botzen WJW, Aerts JCJH, van den Bergh JCJM (2009b) Dependence of flood risk perceptions on socioeconomic and objective risk factors. Water Resour Res. https://doi.org/10.1029/2009WR007743

Article   Google Scholar  

Bourdieu P (2010) Distinction.Bradford R A, O’Sullivan J J, van der Craats I M, Krywkow J, Rotko P, Aaltonen J, Bonaiuto M, De Dominici S, Waylen K, Schelfaut K (2012) Risk perception – issues for flood management in Europe. Nat Hazards Earth Syst Sci 12:2299–2309

Bradford RA, O’Sullivan JJ, van der Craats M, Krywkow J, Rotko P, Aaltonen J, Bonaiuto M, De Dominici S, Waylen K, Schelfaut K (2012) Risk perception – issues for flood management in Europe. Nat Hazards Earth Syst Sci 12:2299–2309

Bubeck P, Botzen WJW, Aerts JCJH (2012) A review of risk perceptions and other factors that influence flood mitigation behavior. Risk Anal 32(9):1481–1495

Burningham K, Fielding J, Thrush D (2008) “It’ll never happen to me”: understanding public awareness of local flood risk. Disasters 32(2):216–238

Burns WJ, Slovic P (2012) Risk perception and behaviors: anticipating and responding to crises. Risk Anal 32:579–582

Burton C, Cutter S (2008) Levee failures and social vulnerability in the Sacramento-San Joaquin Delta Area, California. Nat Hazards Rev 9 (Special issue: Flooding in the Central Valley) 136–149

Burton I, Kates R, Mather JR, Snead RE (1965) The shores of megalopolis: coastal occupance and human adjustment to flood hazard. Publications in climatology 18 (3)

Bustillos Ardaya A, Evers M, Ribbe L (2017) What influences disaster risk perception? Intervention measures, flood and landslide risk perception of the population living in flood risk areas in Rio de Janeiro state, Brazil. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct 25:227–237

Colten CE, Sumpter AR (2009) Social memory and resilience in New Orleans. Nat Hazards 48(3):355–364

Cutter SL, Boruff BJ, Shirley WL (2003) Social vulnerability to environmental hazards. Soc Sci Q 84:242–261

Dake K (1991) Orientating dispositions in the perceptions of risk: an analysis of contemporary worldviews and cultural biases. J of Cross-Cult Psychol 22:61–82

Douglas M, Wildavsky AB (1982) Risk and culture: an essay on the selection of technical and environmental dangers. Berkeley University of California Press, California

Douglas M (1978) Cultural bias. occasional paper no. 35. Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, London

Douglas M. (1992) Risk and Blame. Routledge London

Drake K (1992) Myths of nature: culture and the social construction of risk. J Soc Issues 48(4):21–37

Duží B, Vikhrov D, Kelman I, Stojanov R, Juřička D (2015) Household measures for river flood risk reduction in the Czech Republic. J Flood Risk Manag. https://doi.org/10.1111/jfr3.12132

Działek J, Biernacki W, Bokwa A (2013) Challenges to social capacity building in flood-affected areas of southern Poland. Nat Hazards Earth Syst Sci 13:2555–2566

Działek J, Biernacki W, Bokwa A (2014) Impact of Social capital on local communities’ response to floods in Southern Poland In: Neef A, Shaw R (ed.) Risks and conflicts: local responses to natural disasters (Community, Environment and Disaster Risk Management, Volume 14) Emerald Group Publishing Limited, pp.185 - 205

Englander T, Farago K, Slovic PA (1986) Comparative analysis of risk perception in Hungary and the United states. Soc Behav 1:55–66

Finucane ML, Alhakami A, Slovic P, Johnson SM (2000) The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. J Behav Decis Mak 13:1–17

Fischhoff B, Slovic P, Lichtenstein S, Read S, Combs B (1978) How safe is safe enough - psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sci 9(2):127–152

Fitton SL, Moncaster A, Guthrie P (2015) Investigating the social value of the Ripon rivers flood alleviation scheme. J Flood Risk Manag. https://doi.org/10.1111/jfr3.12176

Flynn J, Slovic P, Mertz CK (1994) Gender, race and environmental health risks. Risk Anal 14:1101–1108

Fox-Rogers L, Devitt C, O’Neill E, Brereton F, Clinch JP (2016) Is there really “nothing you can do”? Pathways to enhanced flood-risk preparedness. J Hydrol 543(Part B):330–343

Fritsche I, Barth M, Jugert P, Masson T, Reese G (2018) A social identity model of pro-environmental action (SIMPEA). Psychol Rev 125:245–269

Giddens A (1979) Central problems in social theory: Action, structure and cntradiction in social analysis

Goszczyńska M, Tyszka T, Slovic P (1991) Risk perception in Poland: a comparison with three other countries. J Beha Deci Mak 4:179–193

Grothmann T, Reusswig F (2006) People at risk of flooding: why some residents take precautionary action while others do not. Nat Hazards 38(1–2):101–120

Gustafson PE (1998) Gender differences in risk perception: theoretical and methodological perspectives. Risk Anal 18(6):805–811

Harries T, Penning-Rowsell E (2011) Victim pressure, institutional inertia and climate change adaptation: the case of flood risk. Glob Environ Change 21:188–197

Heitz C, Spaeter S, Auzet AV, Glatron S (2009) Local stakeholders’ perception of muddy flood risk and implications for management approaches: a case study in Alsace (France). Land Use Policy 26:443–451

Ho C, Shaw D, Lin S, Chiu YC (2008) How do disaster characteristics influence risk perception? Risk Anal 28(3):635–643

Hung HC (2009) The attitude towards flood insurance purchase when respondents’ preferences are uncertain: a fuzzy approach. J Risk Res 12(2):239–258

Johnson EJ, Tversky A (1983) Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. J Pers Soc Psychol 45:20–31

Johnson JG, Wilke A, Weber EU (2004) Beyond a trait view of risk-taking: a domain- specific scale measuring risk perceptions, expected benefits, and perceived-risk attitude in German-speaking populations. Polish Psychol Bull 35:153–172

Karpowicz-Lazreg C, Mullet E (1993) Societal risk as seen by the French public. Risk Anal 13:253–258

Kates RW (1963) Perceptual regions and regional perception in floodplain management. Pap Proc Reg Sci as 11:217–227

Kellens W, Zaalberg R, Neutens T, Vanneuville W, De Maeyer P (2011) An Analysis of the public perception of flood risk on the Belgian Coast. Risk Anal 3(7):1055–1068

Kellens W, Terpstra T, Schelfaut K, De Maeyer P (2013) Perception and communication of flood risks: a literature review. Risk Anal 33(1):24–49

Keown CF (1989) Risk perceptions of Hong Kongese vs. Americans. Risk Anal 9:401–405

Kim DKD, Madison TP (2020) Public risk perception attitude and information-seeking efficacy on floods: a formative study for disaster preparation campaigns and policies. Int J Disaster Risk Sci 11:592–601

King D (2000) You’re on our own: community vulnerability and the need for awareness and education for predictable natural disasters. J Cont Crisis Man 8:223–228

Kleinhesselink RR, and Rosa ER (1994) Nuclear trees in a forest of hazards: a comparison of risk perceptions between American and Japanese university students. In Lowinger C, Hinman G W (eds) Nuclear power at the crossroads: challenges and prospects for the twenty-first century pp. 101–119 International Research Center for Energy and Economic Development, Washington State University

Kleinhesselink RR, Rosa EA (1991) Cognitive representations of risk perceptions: a comparison of Japan and the United States. J Cross-Cult Psychol 22:11–28

Knocke ET, Kolivras KN (2007) Flash flood awareness in south west Virginia. Risk Anal 27(1):155–169

Koerth J, Vafeidis AT, Hinkel J (2017) Household-Level Coastal Adaptation and Its Drivers: A Systematic Case Study Review. Risk Analysis 37(4):629–646

Kousky C, Kunreuther H (2009) Improving flood insurance and flood risk management: insights from St. Louis. Missouri Res Future RFF DP 09–07:1–34

Kreibich H, Thieken AH, Petrow T, Müller M, Merz B (2005) Flood loss reduction of private households due to building precautionary measures: Lessons learned from the Elbe flood in August 2002. Natl Hazards Earth Syst Sci 5:117–126

Kreibich H, Seifert I, Thieken AH, Lindquist E, Wagner K, Merz B (2011) Recent changes in flood preparedness of private households and businesses in Germany. Reg Environ Change 11:59–71

Kuhlicke C, Steinführer A (2013) Searching for resilience or building social capacities for flood risks? Plan Theory Pract 14(1):114–120

Kuhlicke C, Steinführer A, Begg C, Bianchizza C, Bründl M, Buchecker M (2011) Perspectives on social capacity building for natural hazards: outlining an emerging field of research and practice in Europe. Environ Sci Pol 14:804–814

Kuhlicke C, Seebauer S, Hudson P, Begg C, Bubeck P, Dittmer C, Grothmann T, Heidenreich A, Kreibich H, Lorenz D F, Masson T, Reiter J, Thaler T, Thieken A H, Bamberg S (2020) The behavioral turn in flood risk management, its assumptions and potential implications. WIREs Water 7(e1418)

Kunreuther H (1996) Mitigating disaster losses through insurance. J Risk Uncertain 12:171–187

Lamond JE, Proverbs DG, Hammond FN (2009) Accessibility of flood risk insurance in the UK: confusion, competition and complacency. J Risk Res 12(6):825–841

Lechowska E (2018) What determines flood risk perception? A review of factors of flood risk perception and relations between its basic elements. Nat Hazards 94(3):341–1366

Lichtenstein S, Slovi P, Fischhoff B, Layman M, Combs B (1978) Judged frequency of lethal events. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 4:551–578

Lindell MK, Hwang SN (2008) Household’s perceived personal risk and responses in a multihazard environment. Risk Anal 28(2):539–556

Ludy J, Kondolf GM (2012) Flood risk perception on lands ‘protected’ by 100-year Levees. Nat Hazards. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-011-0072-6

De Marchi B (2007) Flood risk management with the public. In: Proceedings of the European Symposium on Flood Risk Management Research pp. 153–154. Istituto di Sociologia Internazionale di Gorizia

Messner F, Meyer V (2006) Flood damage, vulnerability and risk perception – challenges for flood damage research. In: Schanze J, Zeman E, Marsalek J (eds) Flood Risk Management – Hazards, Vulnerability and Mitigation Measures. Nato Science Series IV, Springer, pp. 149–167

Miceli R, Sotgiu I, Settanni M (2008) Disaster preparedness and perception of flood risk: a study in an alpine valley in Italy. J Environ Psychol 28(2):164–173

Morgan MG, Fischhoff B, Bostrom A, Atman C (2001) Risk communication: a mental models approach. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

O’Neill E, Brereton F, Shahumyan H, Clinch JP (2016) The impact of perceived flood exposure on flood-risk perception: the role of distance. Risk Anal 36(11):2158–2186

Oasim S, Khan AN, Shrestha RP, Qasim M (2015) Risk perception of the people in the flood prone Khyber Pukhthunkhwa province of Pakistan. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct 14:373–378

Ober K, Sakdapolrak P (2017) How do social practices shape policy? Analysing the field of ‘ migration as adaptation ’ with Bourdieu’s‘ Theory of Practice.’ Geogr J 183:359–369

Oliver-Smith A (1996) Anthropological research on hazards and disasters. Ann Rev Anthropol 25:303–328

Otway HJ, von Winterfeldt D (1982) Beyond acceptable risk: on the social acceptabilities of technologies. Policy Sci 14:247–256

Pagneux E, Gísladóttir G, Jónsdóttir S (2011) Public perception of flood hazard and flood risk in Iceland: a case study in a watershed prone to ice-jam floods. Nat Hazards 58:269–287

Poortinga W, Bronstering K, Lannon S (2011) Awareness and perceptions of the risks of exposure to indoor radon: a population based approach to evaluate a radon awareness and testing campaign in England and Wales. Risk Anal 31:1800–1812

Poumadere M, Mays C, Slovi, P, Flynn J, Johnson S (1995) What lies behind public acceptance? Comparison of US and French perceptions of the nuclear power option. In: The International Atomic Energy Agency, Proceedings series. International conference on the nuclear power option pp. 393–405 Vienna

Poussin J, Botzen WW, Aerts JC (2014) Factors of influence on flood damage mitigation behaviour by households. Environ Sci Policy 40:69–77

Putnam R D (1993) Making democracy work. Civic traditions in modern Italy. Princeton

Raaijmakers R, Krywkow J, van der Veen A (2008) Flood risk perceptions and spatial multi-criteria analysis: an exploratory research for hazard mitigation. Nat Hazards 46(8):307–322

Raška P (2015) Flood risk perception in Central-Eastern European members states of the EU: a review. Nat Hazards 79:2163–2179

Rayner S, Cantor R (1987) How fair is safe enough. The cultural approach societal technology choice. Risk Anal 7:3–9

Reckwitz A (2002) Toward a theory of social practices. Eur J Soc Theory 5:243–263

Renn O (1989) Risikowahrnehmung – psychologische Determinantenbei der intuitive Erfassung und Bewertung von technischen Risiken. In Eberhard F (ed) Risiko in der Industriegesellschaft: Analyse Vorsorge, Akzeptanz pp. 167–182. Erlangen

Reutera C, Schmida S, Spielhoferb T, Hahne AS (2019) The impact of risk cultures: citizens’ perception of social media use in emergencies across Europe. Technol Forecast Soc Change 148:1–17

Rohrmann B (1994) Risk perception of different societal groups: Australian findings and cross-national comparison. Austral J Psychol 46:150–163

Schelfaut K, Pannemans B, van der Craats I, Krywkow J, Mysiak J, Cools J (2011) Bringing flood resilience into practice: the FREEMAN project. Environ Sci Pol 14:825–833

Scheue S, Haase D, Meyer V (2011) Exploring multicriteria flood vulnerability by integrating economic, social and ecological dimensions of flood risk and coping capacity: from a starting point view towards an end point view of vulnerability. Nat Hazards 58(2):731–751

Schmuck H (2000) “An Act of Allah”: religious explanations for floods in Bangladesh as survival strategy. Int J Mass Emerg Disasters 18(1):85–96

Shaw R, Shiwaku K, Kobayashi H, Kobayashi M (2004) Linking experience, education, perception and earthquake preparedness. Disast Prev Manag 13(1):39–49

Short JF (1984) The social fabric of risk: toward the social transformation of risk analysis. Am Sociol Rev 49:711–725

Siegrist M, Gutscher H (2008) Natural hazards and motivation for mitigation behavior: people cannot predict the affect evoked by a severe flood. Risk Anal 28(3):771–778

Sjöberg L (1996) A discussions of the limitations of the psychometric and cultural theory approaches to risk perception. Radiat Prot Dosim 68:219–225

Sjöberg L (1998) Worry and risk perception. Risk Anal 18:85–93

Slovic P (1987) Perception of Risk. Science 236(4799):280–285

Slovic P, Fischhoff B, Lichtenstein S (1980) Facts and fears: understanding perceived risk. In Schwing R C, Alberts W A Jr (eds) Societal Risk Assessment: How Safe is Safe Enough? pp. 181-216 Plenum Press, New York

Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, Mac Gregor DG (2004) Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: some thoughts about effect, reason, risk and rationality. Risk Anal 24(2):311–322

Slovic P (2000) The perception of risk. Earthscan London

Stojanov R, Duží B, Daněk., Němec D, Procházka D, (2015) Adaptation to the impacts of climate extremes in central europe: a case study in a rural area in the Czech Republic. Sustainability 7(9):12758–12786

Takao., Motoyoshi T, Sato T, Fukuzono T, (2004) Factors determining residents’ preparedness for floods in modern megalopolises: the case of the Tokai flood disaster in Japan. Risk Res 7(7–8):775–787

Teigen KH, Brun W, Slovic P (1988) Societal risks as seen by the Norwegian public. J Behav Decis Mak 1:111–130

Terpstra T (2011) Emotions, trust and perceived risk: affective and cognitive routes to flood preparedness behavior. Risk Anal 31:1658–1675

Terpstra T, Gutteling JM (2008) Households’ perceived responsibilities in flood risk management in the Netherlands. Int J Water Resour Dev 24:555–565

Terpstra T, Lindell MK, Gutteling JM (2009) Does communicating (flood) risk affect (flood) risk perceptions? Results of a quasi experimental study. Risk Anal 29(8):1141–1155

Terpstra T (2009) Flood preparedness: thoughts, feelings and intentions of the Dutch Public. doi: https://doi.org/10.3990/1.9789036529549

Thieken AH, Petrow T, Kreibich H, Merz B (2006) Insurability and mitigation of flood losses in private households in Germany. Risk Anal 26(2):383–395

Thieken AH, Kreibich H, Muller M, Merz B (2007) Coping with floods: preparedness, response and recovery of flood-affected residents in Germany in 2002. Hydrol Sci J 52(5):1016–1037

Thistlethwaite J, Henstra D, Brown C, Scott D (2018) How flood experience and risk perception influences protective actions and behaviours among Canadian Homeowners. Environ Manag 61:197–208

Thompson M, Ellis R, Wildavsky A (1990) Cultural Theory. Westview Press Boulder

Tierney KJ (1999) Toward a critical sociology of risk. Sociol Forum 14(2):215–242

Tobin GA, Montz B (1997) Natural hazards: explanation and integration. The Guilford Press, New York

van Valkengoed AM, Steg L (2019) Meta-analyses of factors motivating climate change adaptation behaviour. Nat Climate Change 9:158–163

Van Zomeren M, Postmes T, Spears R (2008) Toward an integrative social identity model of collective action: a quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspectives. Psychol Bull 134:504–535

Vlek CJH, Stallen PJM (1981) Judging risks and benefits in the small and in the large. Organ Behav Hum Perform 28:235–271

Wachinger G, Renn O, Begg C, Kuhlicke C (2013) The risk perception paradox— implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Anal 33(6):1049–1065

Weichselgartner J (2001) Disaster mitigation: the concept of vulnerability revisited. Disast Prev Manag 10(2):85–94

Werritty A, Houston, D, Ball T, Tavendale A, Black A (2007) Exploring the social impacts of flood risk and flooding in Scotland. Scottish Executive Edinburgh

White G (1945) Human adjustment to floods: a geographical approach to the flood problem in the United States. Research Pape 29 Department of Geography University of Chicago

Wildavsky A, Dake K (1990) Theories of risk perception: who fears what and why. Daedalus 119(4):41–60

Yuan F, Li M, Liu R, Zhai W, Qi B (2021) Social media for enhanced understanding of disaster resilience during Hurricane Florence. Int J Inf Manage 57

Zaalberg R, Midden C, Meijnders A, McCalley T (2009) Prevention, adaptation, and threat denial: flooding experiences in the Netherlands. Risk Anal 29(12):1759–1778

Zhang Y, Hwang SN, Lindell MK (2010) Hazard proximity or risk perception? Evaluating effects of natural and technological hazards on housing values. Environ Behav 42(5):597–624

Download references

This work was supported by the National Science Centre [grant DEC-2011/03/N/HS4/00436].

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Faculty of Economics and Sociology, University of Lodz, 3/5 POW Street, 90-255, Lodz, Poland

Ewa Lechowska

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ewa Lechowska .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

The author has no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose. The author has no conflicts of interest to declare that are relevant to the content of this article. The author certifies that she has no affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest or non-financial interest in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript. The author has no financial or proprietary interests in any material discussed in this article.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Lechowska, E. Approaches in research on flood risk perception and their importance in flood risk management: a review. Nat Hazards 111 , 2343–2378 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-021-05140-7

Download citation

Received : 10 March 2021

Accepted : 24 November 2021

Published : 15 December 2021

Issue Date : April 2022

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-021-05140-7

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Risk perception
  • Rationalist approach
  • Constructivist approach
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Published: 10 August 2021

Causes, impacts and patterns of disastrous river floods

  • Bruno Merz   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-5992-1440 1 , 2 ,
  • Günter Blöschl 3 ,
  • Sergiy Vorogushyn   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-4639-7982 1 ,
  • Francesco Dottori   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1388-3303 4 ,
  • Jeroen C. J. H. Aerts   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-2162-5814 5 , 6 ,
  • Paul Bates 7 ,
  • Miriam Bertola   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-5283-0386 3 ,
  • Matthias Kemter 1 , 2 , 8 ,
  • Heidi Kreibich 1 ,
  • Upmanu Lall   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-0529-8128 9 &
  • Elena Macdonald   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-0198-6556 1  

Nature Reviews Earth & Environment volume  2 ,  pages 592–609 ( 2021 ) Cite this article

16k Accesses

218 Citations

352 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Natural hazards

Disastrous floods have caused millions of fatalities in the twentieth century, tens of billions of dollars of direct economic loss each year and serious disruption to global trade. In this Review, we provide a synthesis of the atmospheric, land surface and socio-economic processes that produce river floods with disastrous consequences. Disastrous floods have often been caused by processes fundamentally different from those of non-disastrous floods, such as unusual but recurring atmospheric circulation patterns or failures of flood defences, which lead to high levels of damage because they are unexpected both by citizens and by flood managers. Past trends in economic flood impacts show widespread increases, mostly driven by economic and population growth. However, the number of fatalities and people affected has decreased since the mid-1990s because of risk reduction measures, such as improved risk awareness and structural flood defences. Disastrous flooding is projected to increase in many regions, particularly in Asia and Africa, owing to climate and socio-economic changes, although substantial uncertainties remain. Assessing the risk of disastrous river floods requires a deeper understanding of their distinct causes. Transdisciplinary research is needed to understand the potential for surprise in flood risk systems better and to operationalize risk management concepts that account for limited knowledge and unexpected developments.

The causative mechanisms of floods with disastrous consequences tend to be different from those of non-disastrous floods, and show anomalies in one or several flood- and loss-generating processes.

Past trends in flood hazard show both upward and downward changes. In some regions, anthropogenic warming is already strong enough to override other drivers of change.

Flood hazards and impacts are projected to increase for many regions around the globe. Future flooding hotspots are expected in Asia and Africa, owing to climate and socio-economic changes.

Reducing vulnerability is a particularly effective way of reducing flood impacts. Global decreases in flood-affected people and fatalities since the mid-1990s (despite a growing population) are signs of effective risk reduction.

Disastrous floods often come as a surprise. Effective risk reduction requires an understanding of the causative processes that make these events distinct and to address the sources of surprise, including cognitive biases.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals

Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription

24,99 € / 30 days

cancel any time

Subscribe to this journal

Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles

92,52 € per year

only 7,71 € per issue

Buy this article

  • Purchase on SpringerLink
  • Instant access to full article PDF

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

research paper on flood management

Similar content being viewed by others

research paper on flood management

Floods and rivers: a circular causality perspective

research paper on flood management

Shifts in flood generation processes exacerbate regional flood anomalies in Europe

research paper on flood management

Changing climate both increases and decreases European river floods

Data availability.

The authors declare that the data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article and its  supplementary information files. Other data can be provided by the authors on request.

Doocy, S., Daniels, A., Murray, S. & Kirsch, T. D. The human impact of floods: a historical review of events 1980−2009 and systematic review. PLOS Curr . https://doi.org/10.1371/currents.dis.f4deb457904936b07c09daa98ee8171a (2013).

United Nations Office for Disaster Risk Reduction GAR2015 — Global Assessment Report on Disaster Risk Reduction . (UNDRR, 2015).

Jongman, B., Ward, P. J. & Aerts, J. C. J. H. Global exposure to river and coastal flooding: long term trends and changes. Glob. Environ. Change 22 , 823–835 (2012).

Article   Google Scholar  

Dottori, F. et al. Increased human and economic losses from river flooding with anthropogenic warming. Nat. Clim. Change 8 , 781–786 (2018).

Barendrecht, M. H., Viglione, A. & Blöschl, G. A dynamic framework for flood risk. Water Secur. 1 , 3–11 (2017).

Vorogushyn, S. et al. Evolutionary leap in large-scale flood risk assessment needed. Wiley Interdisc. Rev. Water 5 , e1266 (2018).

Di Baldassarre, G. et al. Debates — perspectives on socio-hydrology: capturing feedbacks between physical and social processes. Water Resour. Res. 51 , 4770–4781 (2015).

Merz, B. et al. Floods and climate: emerging perspectives for flood risk assessment and management. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 14 , 1921–1942 (2014).

Merz, B., Hall, J., Disse, M. & Schumann, A. Fluvial flood risk management in a changing world. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 10 , 509–527 (2010a).

Chen, Y. et al. Socio-economic impacts on flooding: a 4000-year history of the Yellow River, China. Ambio 41 , 682–698 (2012).

Munoz, S. E. et al. Climatic control of Mississippi River flood hazard amplified by river engineering. Nature 556 , 95–98 (2018).

Blöschl, G. et al. Increasing river floods: fiction or reality? Wiley Interdisc. Rev. Water 2 , 329–344 (2015).

Sharma, A., Wasko, C. & Lettenmaier, D. P. If precipitation extremes are increasing, why aren’t floods? Water Resour. Res. 54 , 8545–8551 (2018).

Ho, M. et al. The future role of dams in the United States of America. Water Resour. Res. 53 , 982–998 (2017).

Kreibich, H. et al. Adaptation to flood risk: results of international paired flood event studies. Earth’s Future 5 , 953–965 (2017).

Lempérière, F. Dams and floods. Engineering 3 , 144–149 (2017).

Kundzewicz, Z. W. et al. Differences in flood hazard projections in Europe — their causes and consequences for decision making. Hydrol. Sci. J. 62 , 1–14 (2017).

Google Scholar  

Tarasova, L. et al. Causative classification of river flood events. Wiley Interdisc. Rev. Water 6 , e1353 (2019).

Smith, J. A., Cox, A. A., Baeck, M. L., Yang, L. & Bates, P. Strange floods: the upper tail of flood peaks in the United States. Water Resour. Res. 54 , 6510–6542 (2018).

Rosbjerg, D. et al. (eds.) In Runoff Prediction in Ungauged Basins 189−226 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2013).

Villarini, G. & Smith, J. A. Flood peak distributions for the eastern United States. Water Resour. Res . 46 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2009wr008395 (2010).

Berghuijs, W. R., Woods, R. A., Hutton, C. J. & Sivapalan, M. Dominant flood generating mechanisms across the United States. Geophys. Res. Lett. 43 , 4382–4390 (2016).

Villarini, G. On the seasonality of flooding across the continental United States. Adv. Water Resour. 87 , 80–91 (2016).

Blöschl, G. et al. Changing climate shifts timing of European floods. Science 357 , 588–590 (2017).

Merz, R. & Blöschl, G. A process typology of regional floods. Water Resour. Res . 39 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2002wr001952 (2003).

Hirschboeck, K. K. Flood hydroclimatology. In Flood Geomorphology (eds Baker, V. R., Kockel, R. C. & Patton, P. C.) 27–49 (John Wiley & Sons, 1988).

Nakamura, J., Lall, U., Kushnir, Y., Robertson, A. W. & Seager, R. Dynamical structure of extreme floods in the U.S. Midwest and the United Kingdom. J. Hydrometeorol. 14 , 485–504 (2013).

Teegavarapu, R. Extreme precipitation and floods. In Floods in a Changing Climate: Extreme Precipitation International Hydrology Series 115−147 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2012).

McGregor, G. R. Climate and rivers. River Res. Appl. 35 , 1119–1140 (2019).

Stohl, A. & James, P. A Lagrangian analysis of the atmospheric branch of the global water cycle. Part I: Method description, validation, and demonstration for the August 2002 flooding in central Europe. J. Hydrometeorol. 5 , 656–678 (2004).

Petvirojchai, P. & SaraPa, S. Current technology for alerting and warning tropical cyclones in Thailand. Trop. Cyclone Res. Rev. 7 , 193–200 (2018).

Francis, J. A. & Vavrus, S. J. Evidence linking Arctic amplification to extreme weather in mid-latitudes. Geophys. Res. Lett . 39 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2012gl051000 (2012).

Hong, C.-C., Hsu, H.-H., Lin, N.-H. & Chiu, H. Roles of European blocking and tropical−extratropical interaction in the 2010 Pakistan flooding. Geophys. Res. Lett . 38 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2011gl047583 (2011).

Grams, C. M., Binder, H., Pfahl, S., Piaget, N. & Wernli, H. Atmospheric processes triggering the central European floods in June 2013. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 14 , 1691–1702 (2014).

Petrow, T., Merz, B., Lindenschmidt, K. E. & Thieken, A. H. Aspects of seasonality and flood generating circulation patterns in a mountainous catchment in south-eastern Germany. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 11 , 1455–1468 (2007).

Payne, A. E. et al. Responses and impacts of atmospheric rivers to climate change. Nat. Rev. Earth Environ. 1 , 143–157 (2020).

Ralph, F. M., Dettinger, M. D., Cairns, M. M., Galarneau, T. J. & Eylander, J. Defining “atmospheric river”: how the glossary of meteorology helped resolve a debate. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 99 , 837–839 (2018).

Dacre, H. F., Clark, P. A., Martinez-Alvarado, O., Stringer, M. A. & Lavers, D. A. How do atmospheric rivers form? Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 96 , 1243–1255 (2015).

Young, A. M., Skelly, K. T. & Cordeira, J. M. High-impact hydrologic events and atmospheric rivers in California: an investigation using the NCEI Storm Events Database. Geophys. Res. Lett. 44 , 3393–3401 (2017).

Barth, N. A., Villarini, G., Nayak, M. A. & White, K. Mixed populations and annual flood frequency estimates in the western United States: the role of atmospheric rivers. Water Resour. Res. 53 , 257–269 (2017).

Lavers, D. A., Villarini, G., Allan, R. P., Wood, E. F. & Wade, A. J. The detection of atmospheric rivers in atmospheric reanalyses and their links to British winter floods and the large-scale climatic circulation. J. Geophys. Res. Atmos . 117 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2012JD018027 (2012).

Eiras-Barca, J. et al. The concurrence of atmospheric rivers and explosive cyclogenesis in the North Atlantic and North Pacific basins. Earth Syst. Dynam. 9 , 91–102 (2018).

Lu, M., Lall, U., Schwartz, A. & Kwon, H. Precipitation predictability associated with tropical moisture exports and circulation patterns for a major flood in France in 1995. Water Resour. Res. 49 , 6381–6392 (2013).

Dhana Lakshmi, D. & Satyanarayana, A. N. V. Influence of atmospheric rivers in the occurrence of devastating flood associated with extreme precipitation events over Chennai using different reanalysis data sets. Atmos. Res. 215 , 12–36 (2019).

Kingston, D. G., Lavers, D. A. & Hannah, D. M. Floods in the southern Alps of New Zealand: the importance of atmospheric rivers. Hydrol. Process. 30 , 5063–5070 (2016).

Porter, K. et al. Overview of the ARkStorm scenario. In U.S. Geological Survey Open-File Report 2010−1312 183 (USGS, 2011).

Porter, K., Cox, D., Dettinger, M. & Ralph, F. M. Special issue on the ARkStorm scenario: California’s other big one. Natural Haz. Rev . 17 , https://doi.org/10.1061/(ASCE)NH.1527-6996.0000234 (2016).

Kundzewicz, Z. W., Szwed, M. & Pińskwar, I. Climate variability and floods — a global review. Water 11 , 1399 (2019).

Waylen, P. R. & Caviedes, C. N. El Nino and annual floods on the north Peruvian littoral. J. Hydrol. 89 , 141–156 (1986).

Cai, W. et al. Climate impacts of the El Niño–Southern Oscillation on South America. Nat. Rev. Earth Environ. 1 , 215–231 (2020).

Kiem, A. S., Franks, S. W. & Kuczera, G. Multi-decadal variability of flood risk. Geophys. Res. Lett . 30 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2002GL015992 (2003).

Ward, P. J., Kummu, M. & Lall, U. Flood frequencies and durations and their response to El Niño Southern Oscillation: global analysis. J. Hydrol. 539 , 358–378 (2016).

Steirou, E., Gerlitz, L., Apel, H., Sun, X. & Merz, B. Climate influences on flood probabilities across Europe. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 23 , 1305–1322 (2019).

Delgado, J. M., Merz, B. & Apel, H. A climate-flood link for the lower Mekong River. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 16 , 1533–1541 (2012).

Messmer, M., Gómez-Navarro, J. J. & Raible, C. C. Climatology of Vb cyclones, physical mechanisms and their impact on extreme precipitation over Central Europe. Earth Syst. Dynam. 6 , 541–553 (2015).

Swierczynski, T. et al. Mid- to late Holocene flood frequency changes in the northeastern Alps as recorded in varved sediments of Lake Mondsee (Upper Austria). Quat. Sci. Rev. 80 , 78–90 (2013).

Hall, J. et al. Understanding flood regime changes in Europe: a state-of-the-art assessment. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 18 , 2735–2772 (2014).

Merz, B., Nguyen, V. D. & Vorogushyn, S. Temporal clustering of floods in Germany: do flood-rich and flood-poor periods exist? J. Hydrol. 541 , 824–838 (2016).

Hodgkins, G. A. et al. Climate-driven variability in the occurrence of major floods across North America and Europe. J. Hydrol. 552 , 704–717 (2017).

Lun, D., Fischer, S., Viglione, A. & Blöschl, G. Detecting flood-rich and flood-poor periods in annual peak discharges across Europe. Water Resour. Res. 56 , e2019WR026575 (2020).

Ward, P. J. et al. Strong influence of El Niño Southern Oscillation on flood risk around the world. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111 , 15659–15664 (2014).

Nobre, G. G., Jongman, B., Aerts, J. & Ward, P. J. The role of climate variability in extreme floods in Europe. Environ. Res. Lett. 12 , 084012 (2017).

Zanardo, S., Nicotina, L., Hilberts, A. G. J. & Jewson, S. P. Modulation of economic losses from European floods by the North Atlantic Oscillation. Geophys. Res. Lett. 46 , 2563–2572 (2019).

Kwon, H.-H., Brown, C. & Lall, U. Climate informed flood frequency analysis and prediction in Montana using hierarchical Bayesian modeling. Geophys. Res. Lett. 35 , L05404 (2008).

Lima, C. H. R., Lall, U., Troy, T. J. & Devineni, N. A climate informed model for nonstationary flood risk prediction: application to Negro River at Manaus, Amazonia. J. Hydrol. 522 , 594–602 (2015).

Schröter, K., Kunz, M., Elmer, F., Mühr, B. & Merz, B. What made the June 2013 flood in Germany an exceptional event? A hydro-meteorological evaluation. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 19 , 309–327 (2015).

Merz, R. & Blöschl, G. Process controls on the statistical flood moments — a data based analysis. Hydrol. Process. 23 , 675–696 (2009).

Norbiato, D., Borga, M., Merz, R., Blöschl, G. & Carton, A. Controls on event runoff coefficients in the eastern Italian Alps. J. Hydrol. 375 , 312–325 (2009).

Bennett, B., Leonard, M., Deng, Y. & Westra, S. An empirical investigation into the effect of antecedent precipitation on flood volume. J. Hydrol. 567 , 435–445 (2018).

Tromp-van Meerveld, H. J. & McDonnell, J. J. Threshold relations in subsurface stormflow: 2. The fill and spill hypothesis. Water Resour. Res . 42 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2004wr003800 (2006).

Rogger, M. et al. Step changes in the flood frequency curve: process controls. Water Resour. Res . 48 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2011WR011187 (2012).

Rogger, M., Viglione, A., Derx, J. & Blöschl, G. Quantifying effects of catchments storage thresholds on step changes in the flood frequency curve. Water Resour. Res. 49 , 6946–6958 (2013).

Blöschl, G., Merz, R. & Reszler, C. Floods in Austria. In Extreme Hydrological Events: New Concepts for Security. NATO Science Series (eds Vasiliev, O., van Gelder, P., Plate, E. & Bolgov, M.) Vol. 78 (Springer, 2006).

Sivapalan, M., Blöschl, G., Merz, R. & Gutknecht, D. Linking flood frequency to long-term water balance: Incorporating effects of seasonality. Water Resour. Res . 41 , https://doi.org/10.1029/2004wr003439 (2005).

Blöschl, G., Nester, T., Komma, J., Parajka, J. & Perdigão, R. A. P. The June 2013 flood in the upper Danube basin, and comparisons with the 2002, 1954 and 1899 floods. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 17 , 5197–5212 (2013).

Guse, B. et al. The role of flood wave superposition in the severity of large floods. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 24 , 1633–1648 (2020).

Mirza, M. M. Q. Global warming and changes in the probability of occurrence of floods in Bangladesh and implications. Glob. Environ. Change 12 , 127–138 (2002).

O’Connell, E., Ewen, J., O’Donnell, G. & Quinn, P. Is there a link between agricultural land-use management and flooding? Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 11 , 96–107 (2007).

Rogger, M. et al. Land use change impacts on floods at the catchment scale: challenges and opportunities for future research. Water Resour. Res. 53 , 5209–5219 (2017).

Bronstert, A. et al. Multi-scale modelling of land-use change and river training effects on floods in the Rhine basin. River Res. Appl. 23 , 1102–1125 (2007).

Te Linde, A. H., Aerts, J. C. J. H. & Kwadijk, J. C. J. Effectiveness of flood management measures on peak discharges in the Rhine basin under climate change. J. Flood Risk Manag. 3 , 248–269 (2010).

Hooijer, A., Klijn, F., Pedroli, G. B. M. & Van Os, A. G. Towards sustainable flood risk management in the Rhine and Meuse riverbasins: synopsis of the findings of IRMA-SPONGE. River Res.Applic. 20 , 343–357 (2004).

Salazar, S. et al. A comparative analysis of the effectiveness of flood management measures based on the concept of “retaining water in the landscape” in different European hydro-climatic regions. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 12 , 3287–3306 (2012).

Pattison, I. & Lane, S. N. The link between land-use management and fluvial flood risk: a chaotic conception? Prog. Phys. Geogr. Earth Environ. 36 , 72–92 (2012).

Yang, L. et al. River networks system changes and its impact on storage and flood control capacity under rapid urbanization. Hydrol. Process. 30 , 2401–2412 (2016).

Best, J. Anthropogenic stresses on the world’s big rivers. Nat. Geosci. 12 , 7–21 (2019).

Van Khanh Triet, N. et al. Has dyke development in the Vietnamese Mekong Delta shifted flood hazard downstream? Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 21 , 3991–4010 (2017).

Remo, J. W. F., Pinter, N. & Heine, R. The use of retro- and scenario-modeling to assess effects of 100+ years river of engineering and land-cover change on Middle and Lower Mississippi River flood stages. J. Hydrol. 376 , 403–416 (2009).

Apel, H., Merz, B. & Thieken, A. H. Influence of dike breaches on flood frequency estimation. Comput. Geosci. 35 , 907–923 (2009).

de Bruijn, K. M., Diermanse, F. L. M. & Beckers, J. V. L. An advanced method for flood risk analysis in river deltas, applied to societal flood fatality risk in the Netherlands. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 14 , 2767–2781 (2014).

Graf, W. L. Downstream hydrologic and geomorphic effects of large dams on American rivers. Geomorphology 79 , 336–360 (2006).

Ayalew, T. B., Krajewski, W. F., Mantilla, R., Wright, D. B. & Small, S. J. Effect of spatially distributed small dams on flood frequency: insights from the Soap Creek watershed. J. Hydrol. Eng. 22 , 04017011 (2017).

Mei, X., Van Gelder, P. H. A. J. M., Dai, Z. & Tang, Z. Impact of dams on flood occurrence of selected rivers in the United States. Front. Earth Sci. 11 , 268–282 (2017).

Wang, W. et al. Nonlinear filtering effects of reservoirs on flood frequency curves at the regional scale. Water Resour. Res. 53 , 8277–8292 (2017).

Volpi, E., Di Lazzaro, M., Bertola, M., Viglione, A. & Fiori, A. Reservoir effects on flood peak discharge at the catchment scale. Water Resour. Res. 54 , 9623–9636 (2018).

Delle Rose, M. Decision-making errors and socio-political disputes over the Vajont dam disaster. Disaster Adv. 5 , 144–152 (2012).

Zscheischler, J. et al. A typology of compound weather and climate events. Nat. Rev. Earth Environ. 1 , 333–347 (2020).

Wahl, T., Jain, S., Bender, J., Meyers, S. D. & Luther, M. E. Increasing risk of compound flooding from storm surge and rainfall for major US cities. Nat. Clim. Change 5 , 093–1097 (2015).

Ganguli, P. & Merz, B. Extreme coastal water levels exacerbate fluvial flood hazards in northwestern Europe. Sci. Rep. 9 , 1–14 (2019).

Couasnon, A. et al. Measuring compound flood potential from river discharge and storm surge extremes at the global scale. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 20 , 489–504 (2020).

Syvitski, J. P. et al. Sinking deltas due to human activities. Nat. Geosci. 2 , 681 (2009).

Ganguli, P., Paprotny, D., Hasan, M., Güntner, A. & Merz, B. Projected changes in compound flood hazard from riverine and coastal floods in northwestern Europe. Earth’s Future 8 , e2020EF001752 (2020).

Douben, K.-J. Characteristics of river floods and flooding: a global overview, 1985–2003. Irrig. Drain. 55 , S9–S21 (2006).

Kreibich, H. et al. Costing natural hazards. Nat. Clim. Change 4 , 303–306 (2014).

Allaire, M. Socio-economic impacts of flooding: a review of the empirical literature. Water Security 3 , 18–26 (2018).

Jonkman, S. N. Global perspectives on loss of human life caused by floods. Natural Haz. 34 , 151–175 (2005).

Hu, P., Zhang, Q., Shi, P. J., Chen, B. & Fang, J. Y. Flood-induced mortality across the globe: spatiotemporal pattern and influencing factors. Sci. Total. Environ. 643 , 171–182 (2018).

Jongman, B. et al. Declining vulnerability to river floods and the global benefits of adaptation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112 , E2271–E2280 (2015).

Jonkman, S. N. & Vrijling, J. K. Loss of life due to floods. J. Flood Risk Manag. 1 , 43–56 (2008).

Ashley, S. T. & Ashley, W. S. Flood fatalities in the United States. J. Appl. Meteorol. Climatol. 47 , 805–818 (2008).

Du, W., FitzGerald, G. J., Clark, M. & Hou, X.-Y. Health impacts of floods. Prehosp. Disaster Med. 25 , 265–272 (2012).

Elliott, J. R. Natural hazards and residential mobility: general patterns and racially unequal outcomes in the United States. Soc. Forces 93 , 1723–1747 (2014).

Moftakhari, H. R., AghaKouchak, A., Sanders, B. F., Allaire, M. & Matthew, R. A. What is nuisance flooding? Defining and monitoring an emerging challenge. Water Resour. Res. 54 , 4218–4227 (2018).

Merz, B., Elmer, F. & Thieken, A. H. Significance of “high probability/low damage” versus “low probability/high damage” flood events. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 9 , 1033–1046 (2009).

World Meteorological Organization. Limpopo River basin: a proposal to improve the flood forecasting and early warning system. (WMO, 2012).

Koks, E. Moving flood risk modelling forwards. Nat. Clim. Change 8 , 561–562 (2018).

Haraguchi, M. & Lall, U. Flood risks and impacts: a case study of Thailand’s floods in 2011 and research questions for supply chain decision making. Int. J. Dis. Risk Reduct. 14 , 256–272 (2015).

World Economic Forum. Global risks 2012 and 2013. (WEF, 2013).

Helbing, D. Globally networked risks and how to respond. Nature 497 , 51–59 (2013).

United Nations Office for Disaster Risk Reduction. Global assessment report on disaster risk reduction. (UNDRR, 2019).

Weiping, W., Yang, S., Stanley, H. E. & Gao, J. Local floods induce large-scale abrupt failures of road networks. Nat. Commun. 10 , 2114 (2019).

Sieg, T. et al. Integrated assessment of short-term direct and indirect economic flood impacts including uncertainty quantification. PLoS ONE 14 , e0212932 (2019).

Thieken, A. H., Müller, M., Kreibich, H. & Merz, B. Flood damage and influencing factors: new insights from the August 2002 flood in Germany. Water Resour. Res. 41 , 1–16 (2005).

Merz, B., Kreibich, H., Schwarze, R. & Thieken, A. Review article “Assessment of economic flood damage”. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 10 , 1697–1724 (2010).

Wing, O. E. J., Pinter, N., Bates, P. D. & Kousky, C. New insights into US flood vulnerability revealed from flood insurance big data. Nat. Commun. 11 , 1444 (2020).

Jonkman, S. N. & Kelman, I. An analysis of the causes and circumstances of flood disaster deaths. Disasters 29 , 75–97 (2005).

Bubeck, P., Botzen, W. J. W., Kreibich, H. & Aerts, J. C. J. H. Long-term development and effectiveness of private flood mitigation measures: an analysis for the German part of the river Rhine. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 12 , 3507–3518 (2012b).

Haer, T., Botzen, W. J. W. & Aerts, J. C. J. H. Advancing disaster policies by integrating dynamic adaptive behaviour in risk assessments using an agent-based modelling approach. Environ. Res. Lett. 14 , 044022 (2019).

Kryspin-Watson, J., Dharmavaram, S., Stanton-Geddes, Z. & Chia, B. Urban Floods Community of Practice. Land use planning for urban flood risk management. (eds Himmelfarb, A. & Mora, J.) (World Bank, 2017).

United Nations International Strategy for Disaster Reduction . Sendai Framework for Disaster Risk Reduction 2015–2030 http://www.unisdr.org/files/43291_sendaiframeworkfordrren.pdf (UNISDR, 2015).

Tasantab, J. C. Beyond the plan: how land use control practices influence flood risk in Sekondi-Takoradi. JÀMBÁ J. Disaster Risk Stud. 11 , 1–9 (2019).

Brody, S. D., Zahran, S., Highfield, W. E., Bernhardt, S. P. & Vedlitz, A. Policy learning for flood mitigation: a longitudinal assessment of the community rating system in florida. Risk Anal. 29 , 912–929 (2009).

Miao, Q. Are we adapting to floods? Evidence from global flooding fatalities. Risk Anal. 39 , 1298–1313 (2019).

Kellens, W., Terpstra, T. & De Maeyer, P. Perception and communication of flood risks: a systematic review of empirical research. Risk Anal. 33 , 24–49 (2013).

Aerts, J. C. J. H. et al. Integrating human behaviour dynamics into flood disaster risk assessment. Nat. Clim. Change 8 , 193–199 (2018).

Kingdon, J. W. Agendas, Alternatives, and Public Policies 2nd edn (Longman, 1995).

Fleming, G. et al. Learning to live with rivers. Report of the Institution of Civil Engineers. https://web.archive.org/web/20110103063637/http://ice.org.uk/Information-resources/Document-Library/Learning-to-live-with-rivers (ICE, 2001).

EU directive 2007/60/EC of the European Parliament and of the Council of 23 October 2007 on the assessment and management of flood risks. The EU Floods Directive https://ec.europa.eu/environment/water/flood_risk (EU, 2007).

Kreibich, H. et al. Recent changes in flood preparedness of private households and businesses in Germany. Reg. Environ. Change 11 , 59–71 (2011).

Gallagher, J. Learning about an infrequent event: evidence from flood insurance take-up in the United States. Am. Econ. J. Appl. Econ. 6 , 206–233 (2014).

Atreya, A., Ferreira, S. & Kriesel, W. Forgetting the flood? An analysis of the flood risk discount over time. Land. Econ. 89 , 577–596 (2013).

Brouwer, R., Akter, S., Brander, L. & Haque, E. Socioeconomic vulnerability and adaptation to environmental risk: a case study of climate change and flooding in Bangladesh. Risk Anal. 27 , 313–326 (2007).

Karim, A. & Noy, I. Poverty and natural disasters: a regression meta-analysis. Rev. Econ. Inst . 7 , https://doi.org/10.5202/rei.v7i2.222 (2016).

Kellenberg, D. K. & Mobarak, A. M. Does rising income increase or decrease damage risk from natural disasters? J. Urban. Econ. 63 , 788–802 (2008).

Fankhauser, S. & McDermott, T. K. J. Understanding the adaptation deficit: why are poor countries more vulnerable to climate events than rich countries? Glob. Environ. Change 27 , 9–18 (2014).

Schumacher, I. & Strobl, E. Economic development and losses due to natural disasters: the role of hazard exposure. Ecol. Econ. 72 , 97–105 (2011).

Muttarak, R. & Lutz, W. Is education a key to reducing vulnerability to natural disasters and hence unavoidable climate change? Ecol. Soc . 19 , 42 (2014).

Milly, P. C. D., Wetherald, R. T., Dunne, K. A. & Delworth, T. L. Increasing risk of great floods in a changing climate. Nature 415 , 514–517 (2002).

Slater, L. et al. Global changes in 20-year, 50-year, and 100-year river floods. Geophys. Res. Lett. 48 , e2020GL091824 (2021).

Blöschl, G. et al. Changing climate both increases and decreases European river floods. Nature 573 , 108–111 (2019).

Slater, L. J. & Villarini, G. Recent trends in U.S. flood risk. Geophys. Res. Lett. 43 , 12428–12436 (2016).

Hirsch, R. M. & Ryberg, K. R. Has the magnitude of floods across the USA changed with global CO 2 levels? Hydrolog. Sci. J. 57 , 1–9 (2012).

Mallakpour, I. & Villarini, G. The changing nature of flooding across the central United States. Nat. Clim. Change 5 , 250–254 (2015).

Archfield, S. A., Hirsch, R. M., Viglione, A. & Blöschl, G. Fragmented patterns of flood change across the United States. Geophys. Res. Lett. 43 , 10232–10239 (2016).

Do, H. X., Westra, S. & Leonard, M. A global-scale investigation of trends in annual maximum streamflow. J. Hydrol. 552 , 28–43 (2017).

Gudmundsson, L., Leonard, M., Do, H. X., Westra, S. & Seneviratne, S. I. Observed trends in global indicators of mean and extreme streamflow. Geophys. Res. Lett. 46 , 756–766 (2019).

Bartiko, D., Oliveira, D. Y., Bonumá, N. B. & Chaffe, P. L. B. Spatial and seasonal patterns of flood change across Brazil. Hydrol. Sci. J. 64 , 1071–1079 (2019).

Ishak, E. & Rahman, A. Examination of changes in flood data in Australia. Water 11 , 1734 (2019).

Yang, L., Wang, L., Li, X. & Gao, J. On the flood peak distributions over China. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 23 , 5133–5149 (2019).

Merz, B., Vorogushyn, S., Uhlemann, S., Delgado, J. & Hundecha, Y. HESS Opinions: “More efforts and scientific rigour are needed to attribute trends in flood time series”. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 16 , 1379–1387 (2012).

Viglione, A. et al. Attribution of regional flood changes based on scaling fingerprints. Water Resour. Res. 52 , 5322–5340 (2016).

Kemter, M., Merz, B., Marwan, N., Vorogushyn, S. & Blöschl, G. Joint trends in flood magnitudes and spatial extents across Europe. Geophys. Res. Lett. 47 , e2020GL087464 (2020).

Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). Managing the risks of extreme events and disasters to advance climate change adaptation. In A Special Report of Working Groups I and II of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (eds Field, C. B. et al.) 582 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2012).

Screen, J. A. & Simmonds, I. Amplified mid-latitude planetary waves favour particular regional weather extremes. Nat. Clim. Change 4 , 704–709 (2014).

Petoukhov, V., Rahmstorf, S., Petri, S. & Schellnhuber, H. J. Quasiresonant amplification of planetary waves and recent Northern Hemisphere weather extremes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110 , 5336–5341 (2013).

Coumou, D., Di Capua, G., Vavrus, S., Wang, L. & Wang, S. The influence of Arctic amplification on mid-latitude summer circulation. Nat. Commun. 9 , 2959 (2018).

Stadtherr, L., Coumou, D., Petoukhov, V., Petri, S. & Rahmstorf, S. Record Balkan floods of 2014 linked to planetary wave resonance. Sci. Adv. 2 , e1501428 (2016).

Delgado, J. M., Apel, H. & Merz, B. Flood trends and variability in the Mekong river. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 14 , 407–418 (2010).

Bertola, M., Viglione, A., Lun, D., Hall, J. & Blöschl, G. Flood trends in Europe: are changes in small and big floods different? Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 24 , 1805–1822 (2020).

Hosking, J. R. M. & Wallis, J. R. Regional Frequency Analysis: an Approach Based on L-Moments (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1997).

Dartmouth Flood Observatory. Space-based Measurement, Mapping, and Modeling of Surface Water For Research, Humanitarian, and Water Resources Applications http://floodobservatory.colorado.edu (DFO, 2020).

EmDAT. The CRED/OFDA International Disaster Database https://www.emdat.be/ (Université Catholique de Louvain, 2020).

DesInventar Sendai. Migrating DesInventar to Sendai Framework format. (United Nations Office for Disaster Risk Reduction, 2020).

SHELDUS. The spatial hazard events and losses database for the United States. Version 19. Center for Emergency Management and Homeland Security https://cemhs.asu.edu/sheldus (Arizona State University, 2020).

Kron, W., Steuer, M., Löw, P., & Wirtz, A. How to deal properly with a natural catastrophe database – analysis of flood losses. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. , 12 , 535–550 https://doi.org/10.5194/nhess-12-535-2012 (2012).

Bouwer, L. M. & Jonkman, S. N. Global mortality from storm surges is decreasing. Environ. Res. Lett. 13 , 014008 (2018).

Di Baldassarre, G. et al. Flood fatalities in Africa: from diagnosis to mitigation. Geophys. Res. Lett . 37 , L22402 https://doi.org/10.1029/2010gl045467 (2010).

Paprotny, D., Sebastian, A., Morales-Nápoles, O. & Jonkman, S. N. Trends in flood losses in Europe over the past 150 years. Nat. Commun. 9 , 1985 (2018).

Bouwer, L. M. Have disaster losses increased due to anthropogenic climate change? Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 92 , 39–46 (2011).

Visser, H., Petersen, A. C. & Ligtvoet, W. On the relation between weather-related disaster impacts, vulnerability and climate change. Clim. Change 125 , 461–477 (2014).

Kriegler, E. et al. The need for and use of socio-economic scenarios for climate change analysis: a new approach based on shared socio-economic pathways. Glob. Environ. Change 22 , 807–822 (2012).

Haer, T., Husby, T. G., Botzen, W. J. W. & Aerts, J. C. J. H. The safe development paradox: an agent-based model for flood risk under climate change in the European Union. Glob. Environ. Change 60 , 102009 (2020).

Ward, P. J. et al. A global framework for future costs and benefits of river-flood protection in urban areas. Nat. Clim. Change 7 , 642 (2017).

Kinoshita, Y., Tanoue, M., Watanabe, S. & Hirabayashi, Y. Quantifying the effect of autonomous adaptation to global river flood projections: application to future flood risk assessments. Environ. Res. Lett. 13 , 014006 (2018).

Willner, S. N., Levermann, A., Zhao, F. & Frieler, K. Adaptation required to preserve future high-end river flood risk at present levels. Sci. Adv . 4 , eaao1914 (2018).

Hirabayashi, Y. et al. Global flood risk under climate change. Nat. Clim. Change 3 , 816−821 (2013).

Winsemius, H. C. et al. Global drivers of future river flood risk. Nat. Clim. Change 6 , 381–385 (2016).

Alfieri, L. et al. Global projections of river flood risk in a warmer world. Earth’s Future 5 , 171–182 (2017).

Jiang, T. et al. Each 0.5 °C of warming increases annual flood losses in China by more than US$60 billion. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 101 , E1464–E1474 (2020).

Dankers, R. et al. First look at changes in flood hazard in the inter-sectoral impact model intercomparison project ensemble. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111 , 3257–3261 (2014).

Arnell, N. W. & Gosling, S. N. The impacts of climate change on river flood risk at the global scale. Clim. Change 134 , 387–401 (2016).

Do, H. X. et al. Historical and future changes in global flood magnitude — evidence from a model–observation investigation. Hydrol. Earth Syst. Sci. 24 , 1543–1564 (2020).

Alfieri, L., Dottori, F., Betts, R., Salamon, P. & Feyen, L. Multi-model projections of river flood risk in Europe under global warming. Climate 6 , 6 (2018).

Shepherd, T. G. et al. Storylines: an alternative approach to representing uncertainty in physical aspects of climate change. Clim. Change 151 , 555–571 (2018).

Pfahl, S., O’Gorman, P. A. & Fischer, E. M. Understanding the regional pattern of projected future changes in extreme precipitation. Nat. Clim. Change 7 , 423–427 (2017).

Knutti, R., Masson, D. & Gettelman, A. Climate model genealogy: generation CMIP5 and how we got there. Geophys. Res. Lett. 40 , 1194–1199 (2013).

Scussolini, P. et al. FLOPROS: an evolving global database of flood protection standards. Natural Haz. Earth Syst. Sci. 16 , 1049–1061 (2016).

Jonkman, S. N. Advanced flood risk analysis required. Nat. Clim. Change 3 , 1004–1004 (2013).

Huizinga, J., de Moel, H. & Szewczyk, W. Global flood depth-damage functions. Methodology and the database with guidelines. EUR 28552 EN (European Union, 2017).

Sairam, N., Schröter, K., Lüdtke, S., Merz, B. & Kreibich, H. Quantifying flood vulnerability reduction via private precaution. Earth’s Future 7 , 235–249 (2019).

Lumbroso, D. Flood risk management in Africa. J. Flood Risk Manag. 13 , e12612 (2020).

March, J. G., Sproull, L. S. & Tamuz, M. Learning from samples of one or fewer. Org. Sci. 2 , 1–13 (1991).

Burton, I. Forensic disaster investigations in depth: a new case study model. Environ. Sci. Policy Sustain. Dev. 52 , 36–41 (2010).

Ciullo, A., Viglione, A., Castellarin, A., Crisci, M. & Di Baldassarre, G. Socio-hydrological modelling of flood-risk dynamics: comparing the resilience of green and technological systems. Hydrolog. Sci. J. 62 , 880–891 (2017).

Berner, C. L., Staid, A., Flage, R. & Guikema, S. D. The use of simulation to reduce the domain of “black swans” with application to hurricane impacts to power systems. Risk Anal. 37 , 1879–1897 (2017).

Wardekker, J. A., de Jong, A., Knoop, J. M. & van der Sluijs, J. P. Operationalising a resilience approach to adapting an urban delta to uncertain climate changes. Technol. Forecast. Soc. Change 77 , 987–998 (2010).

Merz, B., Vorogushyn, S., Lall, U., Viglione, A. & Blöschl, G. Charting unknown waters — on the role of surprise in flood risk assessment and management. Water Resour. Res. 51 , 6399–6416 (2015).

Parker, W. S. & Risbey, J. S. False precision, surprise and improved uncertainty assessment. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. A 373 , 20140453 (2015).

Jain, S. & Lall, U. Floods in a changing climate: does the past represent the future? Water Resour. Res. 37 , 3193–3205 (2001).

Leonard, M. et al. A compound event framework for understanding extreme impacts. Wiley Interdisc. Rev. Clim. Change 5 , 113–128 (2014).

Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow 499 (Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011).

Woo, G. Downward counterfactual search for extreme events. Front. Earth Sci . https://doi.org/10.3389/feart.2019.00340 (2019).

Brysse, K., Oreskes, N., O’Reilly, J. & Oppenheimer, M. Climate change prediction: erring on the side of least drama? Glob. Environ. Change 23 , 327–337 (2013).

Croskerry, P., Singhal, G. & Mamede, S. Cognitive debiasing. 1: Origins of bias and theory of debiasing. BMJ Qual. Saf. 22 , ii58–ii64 (2013).

Taleb, N. N. The Black Swan: The Impact of the Highly Improbable 444 (Random House, 2007).

Ben-Haim, Y. Why risk analysis is difficult, and some thoughts on how to proceed. Risk Anal. 32 , 1638–1646 (2012).

IRGC. Introduction to the IRGC Risk Governance Framework, revised version 2017. (EPFL International Risk Governance Center, 2017).

Kwakkel, J. H., Walker, W. E. & Haasnoot, M. Coping with the wickedness of public policy problems: approaches for decision making under deep uncertainty. J. Water Resour. Plan. Manag. 142 , 01816001 (2016).

Simpson, M. et al. Decision analysis for management of natural hazards. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 41 , 489–516 (2016).

Lempert, R. et al. Ensuring Robust Flood Risk Management in Ho Chi Minh City. World Bank Policy Research Working Paper No. 6465 https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2271955 (SSRN, 2013).

Kalra, N. et al. Agreeing on robust decisions: new processes for decision making under deep uncertainty. Policy Research Working Paper No. 6906. 35 (World Bank, 2014).

Sayers, P. et al. Flood Risk Management: A Strategic Approach (UNESCO, 2013).

Haasnoot, M., Kwakkel, J. H., Walker, W. E. & ter Maat, J. Dynamic adaptive policy pathways: a method for crafting robust decisions for a deeply uncertain world. Glob. Environ. Change 23 , 485–498 (2013).

Korteling, B., Dessai, S. & Kapelan, Z. Using information-gap decision theory for water resources planning under severe uncertainty. Water Resour. Manag. 27 , 1149–1172 (2013).

Brown, C., Ghile, Y., Laverty, M. & Li, K. Decision scaling: linking bottom-up vulnerability analysis with climate projections in the water sector. Water Resour. Res. 48 , W09537 (2012).

Restemeyer, B., van den Brink, M. & Woltjer, J. Resilience unpacked — framing of ‘uncertainty’ and ‘adaptability’ in long-term flood risk management strategies for London and Rotterdam. Eur. Plan. Stud. 26 , 1559–1579 (2018).

Disse, M., Johnson, T. G., Leandro, J. & Hartmann, T. Exploring the relation between flood risk management and flood resilience. Water Secur. 9 , 100059 (2020).

Sen, P. K. Estimates of the regression coefficient based on Kendall’s tau. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 63 , 1379–1389 (1968).

Kriegler, E., et al. Fossil-fueled development (SSP5): An energy and resource intensive scenario for the 21st century. Global Environmental Change 42 , 297–315 (2017).

Gall, M., Borden, K. A. & Cutter, S. L. When do losses count?: Six fallacies of natural hazards loss data. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 90 , 799–810 (2009).

Panwar, V. & Sen, S. Disaster damage records of EM-DAT and DesInventar: a systematic comparison. Econ. Dis. Clim. Change 4 , 295–317 (2020).

Adelekan, I. O. Flood risk management in the coastal city of Lagos, Nigeria. J. Flood Risk Manag. 9 , 255–264 (2016).

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the DFG projects ‘SPATE’ (FOR 2416) and ‘NatRiskChange’ (GRK 2043/1), the FWF ‘SPATE’ project (I 3174), the ERC Advanced Grant ‘FloodChange’ project (number 291152), the Horizon 2020 ETN ‘System Risk’ project (number 676027) and the Helmholtz Climate Initiative. P.B. was supported by a Royal Society Wolfson Research Merit award. J.C.J.H.A. was supported by an ERC Advanced Grant COASTMOVE (number 884442) and a NWO-VICI grant (number 453-13-006).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Section Hydrology, Helmholtz Centre Potsdam, GFZ German Research Centre for Geosciences, Potsdam, Germany

Bruno Merz, Sergiy Vorogushyn, Matthias Kemter, Heidi Kreibich & Elena Macdonald

Institute for Environmental Sciences and Geography, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

Bruno Merz & Matthias Kemter

Institute of Hydraulic Engineering and Water Resources Management, Vienna University of Technology, Vienna, Austria

Günter Blöschl & Miriam Bertola

European Commission, Joint Research Centre, Ispra, Italy

Francesco Dottori

Institute for Environmental Studies (IVM), Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Jeroen C. J. H. Aerts

Deltares, Delft, The Netherlands

School of Geographical Sciences, Bristol University, Bristol, UK

Potsdam Institute for Climate Impact Research (PIK), Potsdam, Germany

Matthias Kemter

Earth and Environmental Engineering, Colombia Water Center, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA

Upmanu Lall

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

B.M. suggested the original concept and coordinated the writing. G.B., S.V. and F.D. made major contributions to the writing. B.M., M.K., E.M. and S.V. generated the figures. All authors discussed the concepts and contributed to the writing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bruno Merz .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information.

Nature Reviews Earth & Environment thanks Seth Westra and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information.

(AAL). A widespread indicator for risk, it is the estimated average loss per year considering the full range of scenarios from frequent events (zero or small loss) to extreme events (large loss or worst-case scenario).

Fall of rain onto existing snow, leading to flood runoff composed of snowmelt and rainfall.

Long, narrow and transient corridors of strong horizontal water vapour, transporting on average more than double the flow of the Amazon river and delivering moisture as heavy precipitation.

The fraction of the event water input (precipitation or snowmelt within the catchment) that is not retained in the catchment and that directly contributes to discharge during the event.

Consequences occurring in the inundated region during a flooding event.

Consequences occurring far away from the flooded region and/or after a flooding event.

Consequences of a flooding event that are difficult or impossible to monetarize, such as loss of life or loss of memorabilia.

The ratio of the number of people who lose their lives in a flood to the number of people affected by the flooding event.

The highest streamflow peak in each year.

The dates of the year when floods occur.

The distance over which flooding occurs simultaneously.

Level at which a flood causes extensive inundation, significant evacuations, or property transfer to higher ground.

Level at which a flood does not cause damage but requires mitigation action in preparation for more substantial flooding.

According to the DFO, either the total number of people left homeless after the incident, or the number of people evacuated during the flood.

Coupled Model Intercomparison Project Phase 5; for coordinated climate change experiments for the Fifth Assessment Report AR5 of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change and beyond.

An indicator expressing the exceedance probability or rarity of an event. For instance, a 100-year flood discharge has a probability of 1/100 of being exceeded in a given year.

Relation between flood discharge and the associated return period.

Optimizing risk reduction measures based on the best available knowledge.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Merz, B., Blöschl, G., Vorogushyn, S. et al. Causes, impacts and patterns of disastrous river floods. Nat Rev Earth Environ 2 , 592–609 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s43017-021-00195-3

Download citation

Accepted : 16 June 2021

Published : 10 August 2021

Issue Date : September 2021

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s43017-021-00195-3

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Flood teleconnections from levees undermine disaster resilience.

  • Abolfazl Hojjat Ansari
  • Alfonso Mejia

npj Natural Hazards (2024)

Higher emissions scenarios lead to more extreme flooding in the United States

  • Hanbeen Kim
  • Gabriele Villarini

Nature Communications (2024)

High-resolution impact-based early warning system for riverine flooding

  • Husain Najafi
  • Pallav Kumar Shrestha
  • Luis Samaniego

Stochastic coastal flood risk modelling for the east coast of Africa

  • Irene Benito

Historical trends and future implications of disasters in Honduras

  • Adolfo Quesada-Román
  • Lidia Torres-Bernhard
  • Natalia Martínez-Rojas

Natural Hazards (2024)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

research paper on flood management

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

sustainability-logo

Article Menu

research paper on flood management

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Long-term variation patterns of precipitations driven by climate change in china from 1901 to 2022.

research paper on flood management

1. Introduction

2. materials and methods, 2.1. study area, 2.2. data source and preprocessing, 2.3. research methods, 2.3.1. technical flowchart, 2.3.2. mann–kendall trend test, 2.3.3. mann–kendall mutation test, 2.3.4. center of gravity model, 2.3.5. validation of worldclim data, 3.1. spatial distribution of precipitation during 1901–2022, 3.2. temporal variations in precipitation during 1901–2022, 3.3. distribution characteristics of the centers of gravity in different basins, 4. discussion, 5. conclusions, author contributions, institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

  • Yao, H.X.; Li, Q.Q.; Zhao, L.; Wu, X.Q.; Shen, X.Y.; Duan, C.F.; Li, C. Evolution characteristics of compound drought and heat events during the warm season in the Huaihe River Basin and their relationship with climate and vegetation. Acta Ecol. Sin. 2024 , 44 , 5596–5608. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhao, Z.C.; Luo, Y.; Huang, J.B. Global climate indicators, climatic-impact drivers and global warming. Clim. Chang. Res. 2024 , 20 , 384–388. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liang, P.D.; Liu, A.X.; Duan, L.Y.; Zhou, M.S.; Zhou, L.D. The Relationship between the Persistent Anomaly of Spring and Summer Atmospheric Circulation in the Middle Part of Asia and the Summer Drought/Waterlogging in Eastern China. Chin. J. Atmos. Sci. 2008 , 32 , 1174–1186. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Xu, D.P.; Li, J.M.; Zhou, Z.H.; Liu, J.J.; Yan, Z.Q.; Wang, D.X. Study on the spatial and temporal distribution of precipitation characteristics in China from 1956 to 2018. Water Resour. Hydropower Eng. 2020 , 51 , 20–27. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen, S.; Zhao, W.W.; Han, Y. Spatio-temporal variation of vegetation precipitation use efficiency and influencing factors in arid and semi-arid areas of China. Acta Ecol. Sin. 2023 , 43 , 10295–10307. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yang, Z.H.; Tuo, Y.; Yang, J.; Wu, Y.Z.; Gong, Z.Q.; Feng, G.L. Integrated prediction of summer in China based on multi dynamic-statistic methods. Chin. J. Geophys. 2024 , 67 , 982–996. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu, X.P.; Tong, X.H.; Jia, Q.Y.; Xin, Z.H.; Yang, J.R. Precipitation concentration characteristics in China during 1960–2017. Adv. Water Sci. 2021 , 32 , 10–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang, L.P.; Wang, Y.Z.; Xiang, X.; Sun, H.; Lian, Z.H. Research on classification of short-duration heavy rain based on Gumbel-Linear Moment and K-Means algorithm. Chin. J. Geophys. 2023 , 66 , 3171–3184. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hu, Y.Y.; Xiao, Y.; Dai, S.P.; Luo, H.X.; Li, Y.P.; Li, M.F. Spatio-temporal Variation Characteristics of Precipitation and Temperature in Tropical China from 1961 to 2015. Southwest China J. Agric. Sci. 2021 , 34 , 1788–1795. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lu, S.; Hu, Z.Y.; Wang, B.P.; Qin, P.; Wang, L. Spatio-temporal Patterns of Extreme Precipitation Events over China in Recent 56 Years. Plateau Meteorol. 2020 , 39 , 683–693. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deng, H.J.; Guo, B.; Cao, Y.Q.; Chen, Z.S.; Zhang, Y.Q.; Chen, X.W.; Gao, L.; Chen, Y.; Liu, M.B. Spatial and temporal patterns of daytime and nighttime precipitation in China during 1961–2016. Geogr. Res. 2020 , 39 , 2415–2426. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhao, Z.H.; Luo, Z.J.; Huang, L.X.; Xing, L.T.; Sun, H.J.; Chen, H.L.; Sun, B. Analysis of Precipitation and Groundwater Variation Based on STL and Mann-Kendall Methods in Jinan City. J. China Hydrol. 2022 , 42 , 73–77. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oliver, J.E. Monthly precipitation distribution: A comparative index. Prof. Geogr. 1980 , 32 , 300–309. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Michiels, P.; Gabriels, D.; Hartmann, R. Using the seasonal and temporal Precipitation concentration index for characterizing the monthly rainfall distribution in Spain. Catena 1992 , 19 , 43–58. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Javier, M. Spatial distribution of a daily precipitation concentration index in peninsular Spain. Int. J. Climatol. 2004 , 24 , 959–971. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kong, F.; Shi, P.J.; Fang, J.; Lu, L.L.; Fang, J.Y.; Guo, J.P. Advances and Prospects of Spatiotemporal Pattern Variation of Extreme Precipitation and its Affecting Factors under the Background of Global Climate Change. J. Catastrophol. 2017 , 32 , 165–174. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu, K.; Nie, G.G.; Zhang, S. Study on the spatiotemporal evolution of temperature and precipitation in China from 1951 to 2018. Adv. Earth Sci. 2020 , 35 , 1113–1126. [ Google Scholar ]
  • He, Q.X.; Chen, C.F.; Wang, Y.H.; Sun, Y.N.; Liu, Y.T.; Hu, B.J. Fusion Method for Multi-Source Remote Sensing Daily Precipitation Data: Random Forest Model Considering Spatial Autocorrelation. J. Geo-Inf. Sci. 2024 , 26 , 1517–1530. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang, R.N.; Sun, C.H.; Li, W.J. Relationship between the interannual variations of Arctic sea ice and summer Eurasian teleconnection and associated on summer precipitation over China. Chin. J. Geophys. 2018 , 61 , 91–105. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang, L.J.; Qian, Y.F. Annual distribution features of precipitation in China and their interannual variations. Acta Meteorol. Sin. 2003 , 17 , 146–163. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peng, S.Z. 1-km monthly precipitation dataset for China (1901–2023). Natl. Tibet. Plateau/Third Pole Environ. Data Cent. 2020 , 12 , 15–24. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang, D. Study on the Characteristics of Nighttime Light Distribution and Urban Gravity Center Migration in Urumqi. J. Xinjiang Norm. Univ. 2021 , 40 , 29–34+51. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yan, J.; Li, H.; Wang, J.N. Prediction of the Spatio-Temporal Variation Characteristics of Precipitation in Central Asia Based on the CMIP6 Model. Sci. Technol. Inf. 2024 , 22 , 244–248. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gao, X.T.; Dong, J.; Yang, R.R. Temporal and Spatial Variation of Precipitation in North China During the Last 50 Years. Henan Sci. 2016 , 34 , 596–600. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu, M.R.; Liu, S.T.; Ma, C.Y.; Li, H.; Chang, S.H.; Hou, F.J.; Liu, Y.J. Research progress of the responses of grassland plants and soil to the variation of temperature and precipitation. Chin. J. Ecol. 2023 , 8 , 125–137. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang, X.; Li, P.; Li, D. Spatiotemporal variations of precipitation in the southern part of the Heihe river basin (China), 1984–2014. Water 2018 , 10 , 410. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lv, J.X.; Liu, C.M.; Liang, K.; Tian, W.; Bai, P.; Zhang, Y.H. Spatiotemporal variations of extreme precipitation in the Yellow River Basin based on water resources regionalization. Resour. Sci. 2022 , 44 , 261–273. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wang, L.Z.; Miao, J.F.; Han, F.R. Overview of Impact of Topography on Precipitation in China over Last 10 Years. Meteorol. Sci. Technol. 2018 , 46 , 64–75. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Du, Y.B.; Li, S.S.; Feng, D.; Xiao, Y.Q.; Chen, X.T.; Huang, S.N.; Du, L.L. Characteristics of Extreme Summer Precipitation and Large-Scale Cir-culation in Shaanxi Province under Global Warming. Plateau Meteorol. 2024 , 43 , 342–352. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang, Q.F.; Chen, M.; Qi, X.H. Analysis of Multi-Year Precipitation and Evaporation Changes in Fujin City from 1992 to 2020. Water Resour. Dev. Manag. 2024 , 10 , 70–73+69. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang, Y.Y.; Xin, C.L.; Guo, X.Y.; Zhang, B.; Chen, N.; Shi, Y.F. Characteristics of Stable Isotopes in Precipitation and Its Moisture Sources in the Inland Regions of Northwest China. Environ. Sci. 2024 , 45 , 2080–2095. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lang, L.C.; Tang, C.; Gao, X.; Li, Z.H.; Wu, F. Spatial interpolation of high-resolution daily precipitation over complex terrains. Acta Geogr. Sin. 2023 , 78 , 101–120. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tang, W.R.; Fang, K.Y.; Mei, Z.P.; Wu, H.; Zhou, F.F.; Chen, Y.; Li, W.L. The influence of climate change on social evolution in Eurasia over the past millennium. Chin. Sci. Bull. 2024 , 1–12. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Li, X.E.; Liu, Y.; Jiang, L.M.; Huang, R.G.; Zhou, Z.W.; Pang, X.G. A Review on Remote Sensing Monitoring of Glacial Lake Change and Glacial Lake Outburst Floods in Mountain Glacier Regions. J. Geo-Inf. Sci. 2024 , 26 , 1019–1039. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang, Y.R.; Zhou, D.G.; Guo, X.F. Regional climate response to global warming in the source region of the Yellow River and its impact on runoff. Sci. China Earth Sci. 2024 , 54 , 862–873. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Liu, J.X.; Zhu, Z.W.; Lu, R.; Li, J. Objective Classification of Sea Surface Temperature Evolution diversity of ENSO Cycle. Plateau Meteorol. 2024 , 4 , 87–102. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yang, B.L. Analysis of Runoff Change Characteristics and Driving Factors in the Upper Reaches of the Meijiang River Basin. Guangdong Water Resour. Hydropower 2022 , 7 , 43–47. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen, L.J.; Wang, Y.Y.; Li, W.J.; Sun, L.H.; Li, X.; Zhang, D.Q. Review of the Influence and Application of SST Anomaly to Flood Season Precipitation in China. J. Appl. Meteorol. Sci. 2024 , 35 , 129–141. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hao, Z.C.; Chen, Y. Research progresses and prospects of multi-sphere compound extremes from the Earth System perspective. Sci. China Earth Sci. 2024 , 67 , 343–374. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wang, J.W.; Ran, J.L.; Wu, W.; Shang, R.Y.; Zhang, Z.M.; Zhang, M.X.; Liu, J.K. Scaling Effects of Anthropogenic and Climate Change Impacts on Runoff in the Haihe River Basin. J. Ecol. Rural. Environ. 2024 , 40 , 757–765. [ Google Scholar ]

Click here to enlarge figure

Standard Deviation Ellipse ParameterCorner Angle (°)Standard Deviation along the x-Axis (km)Standard Deviation along the y-Axis (km)Area
(km )
Yangtze River basin86.53614.0034.993219.653
Southeast rivers159.1351.9365.07630.868
Haihe River basin28.6212.6266.98957.649
Huaihe River basin27.7661.9978.56253.712
Yellow River basin73.62914.5552.07294.766
Liaohe River basin108.7756.7252.53353.520
Songhua River basin136.2823.5868.62297.122
Northwest rivers77.14328.81815.1391370.655
Southwest rivers126.56311.5513.268118.604
Pearl River basin101.1619.8233.962122.268
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Han, J.; Zhang, R.; Guo, B.; Han, B.; Xu, T.; Guo, Q. Long-Term Variation Patterns of Precipitations Driven by Climate Change in China from 1901 to 2022. Sustainability 2024 , 16 , 7283. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16177283

Han J, Zhang R, Guo B, Han B, Xu T, Guo Q. Long-Term Variation Patterns of Precipitations Driven by Climate Change in China from 1901 to 2022. Sustainability . 2024; 16(17):7283. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16177283

Han, Jing, Rui Zhang, Bing Guo, Baomin Han, Tianhe Xu, and Qiang Guo. 2024. "Long-Term Variation Patterns of Precipitations Driven by Climate Change in China from 1901 to 2022" Sustainability 16, no. 17: 7283. https://doi.org/10.3390/su16177283

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

IMAGES

  1. Flood Research Paper 1

    research paper on flood management

  2. (PDF) IMPACTS OF FLOOD AND ITS MANAGEMENT

    research paper on flood management

  3. Overview

    research paper on flood management

  4. (PDF) An Introduction to Flood Risk and Management

    research paper on flood management

  5. (PDF) Methods of assessing flood resilience of critical buildings

    research paper on flood management

  6. (PDF) Reducing Flooding Impacts to the Built Environment: A Literature

    research paper on flood management

COMMENTS

  1. A review of the flood management: from flood control to flood

    Flood management is widely recognized as an effective way to reduce the adverse consequences, and a more resilient and sustainable flood management approach has been the goal in recent studies. This study used a detailed bibliometric analysis of keywords, terms and timelines in the research field of the flood research.

  2. (PDF) Flood Disaster Hazards; Causes, Impacts and Management: A State

    the w orst flooding in 30 years that ba ttered W est Africa from July 200 7 caused more. than 21 0 death and aff ected more than 78 5,000 people [ 12]. The aftermaths of flood disasters in Ghana ...

  3. Journal of Flood Risk Management

    Journal of Flood Risk Management provides an international platform for knowledge sharing in all areas related to flood risk. We publish flood related research with content ranging from leading edge academic papers to innovative applied content with the practitioner in mind. Our readers and authors come from a wide background and share an ...

  4. (PDF) A review of the flood management: from flood control to flood

    a. Key Laboratory of Urban Environment and Health, Institute of Urban Environment, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Xiamen 361021, China. b. University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049 ...

  5. Preparing for severe flooding: Flood risk management research leading

    Journal of Flood Risk Management provides an international platform for knowledge sharing in all areas related to flood risk - from academic papers to applied content. Skip to Article Content; Skip to Article Information; Search ... Research into natural flood risk management approaches is also providing improved guidance to modelling ...

  6. Flood risk management through a resilience lens

    Fig. 1: Adopting a resilience lens by operationalizing the four elements into an integrated flood risk management approach. A welfare and recovery capacity (element 1 and 2): Different effects of ...

  7. A Systematic Analysis of Systems Approach and Flood Risk Management

    Flooding is a global threat, necessitating a comprehensive management approach. Due to the complexity of managing flood hazards and risks, researchers have advocated for holistic, comprehensive, and integrated approaches. This study, employing a systems thinking perspective, assessed global flood risk management research trends, gaps, and opportunities using 132 published documents in BibTeX ...

  8. A Critical Review of Flood Risk Management and the Selection of ...

    The research also discusses how flood risk evolves, what the causes of this development are, how they can be affected so as to mitigate the risks (by what points of attack and by what measures), and how these can be combined. ... Integrated Flood Management Concept Paper; World Meteorological Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2009. [Google ...

  9. Floods and rivers: a circular causality perspective

    This paper proposes a framework to investigate the circular causality of channel and floods dynamics, to identify those variables that take part in the behaviour of the river system. It aims to ...

  10. Urban flood risk management needs nature-based solutions: a coupled

    These studies were first checked to select peer-reviewed papers written in English and exclude non-research papers. Based on this, 2636 studies remained for review.

  11. Urban Flood Resilience: A comprehensive review of ...

    This paper conducts a comprehensive and systematic literature review on urban flood resilience assessment (UFRA) to address the inadequacy of urban preparedness, response and lengthy recovery process. ... To enhance the outcomes of the flood management process, empirical research based on spatial layout planning, landscape performance studies ...

  12. A systematic review of natural flood management modelling: Approaches

    Journal of Flood Risk Management provides an international platform for knowledge sharing in all areas related to flood risk - from academic papers to applied content. Abstract The Pitt Review of the 2007 summer floods in the UK, published in 2008, commended the potential of natural flood management (NFM) for reducing flood risk.

  13. Flood Risk in Urban Areas: Modelling, Management and Adaptation to

    The goal of the current paper is to provide a general review of the recent advances in flood-risk modelling and management, while also exploring future perspectives in these fields of research. The modelling and management of flood risk in urban areas are increasingly recognized as global challenges.

  14. A review of the community flood risk management literature ...

    This study systematically reviews the diverse body of research on community flood risk management in the USA to identify knowledge gaps and develop innovative and practical lessons to aid flood management decision-makers in their efforts to reduce flood losses. The authors discovered and reviewed 60 studies that met the selection criteria (e.g., study is written in English, is empirical ...

  15. Approaches in research on flood risk perception and their ...

    The paper concludes by pointing out the desired direction of research on flood risk perception from the perspective of improving flood risk management. In contemporary empirical works managing the perception of flood risk, a rationalistic approach that psychometrically searches for cognitive models dominates.

  16. (PDF) Flood risk management strategies across boundaries: a research

    Flood Risk Management Strategies (FRMSs) are designed t o make vulnerable urban regions more resilient to flooding. This likely requires. changes in their institutional embedding. Insights in to ...

  17. A methodology for flood susceptibility and vulnerability analysis in

    Nowadays, flood risk management is gaining importance in order to mitigate and prevent flood disasters, and consequently the analysis of flood risk components is becoming a key research topic. In this paper, we propose a methodology for large-scale analysis of flood susceptibility and vulnerability.

  18. Issues and Challenges in Flood Risk Management—Editorial for the

    Recent flood-related disasters (Japan, Thailand, US, Australia) emphasize the need for an effective management of flood risks. As an introduction to this special issue, this editorial summarizes some of the key challenges in the field. Flood risk management needs to recognize the interconnections between infrastructures, economic systems and the role of human factors in assessing and managing ...

  19. Causes, impacts and patterns of disastrous river floods

    The UK floods in 2000 137 and the central Europe floods in 2002 15 were perceived as signal to society and policy, and triggered wide-ranging changes in flood risk management, including the EU ...

  20. STUDY AND EVALUATION OF VARIOUS FLOOD MANAGEMENT FRAMEWORKS

    The paper focuses on the various methods of flood management in a systematic and logical manner starting from the main goal, problem identification, risk assessment, data collection, model ...

  21. (PDF) An Overview of Flood Risk Analysis Methods

    This research aims to create an overview of flood analysis and forecasting methods. The study is based on the need to select and group papers into well-defined methodological categories. The ...

  22. Sustainability

    Studying long-term precipitation trends is crucial for sustainable development, as the proper utilization of water resources is essential for maintaining a sustainable water supply. The objective and novelty of this paper was to reveal the gradual mutation process of precipitation in China over a century. This study utilized monthly precipitation data from 1901 to 2022 (at a century scale) to ...

  23. (PDF) Review of Flood Management Techniques

    The present paper tries to investigate the potential flood mapping techniques. Conventionally use of traditional. methods is adapted for flood plain mapping. But this paper focuses on the use of ...